Zoosyst. Evol. 96 (2) 2020, 515-525 | DO! 10.3897/zse.96.38770 Ate BERLIN A guild classification system proposed for anuran advertisement calls Mike Emmrich!, Miguel Vences*, Raffael Ernst?, Jorn Kéhler*, Michael F. Barej', Frank Glaw?, Martin Jansen®, Mark-Oliver Rédel! Museum fiir Naturkunde, Leibniz Institute for Evolution and Biodiversity Science, Invalidenstr. 43, 10115 Berlin, Germany Zoological Institute, Technische Universitat Braunschweig, Mendelssohnstr. 4, 38106 Braunschweig, Germany Museum fir Tierkunde, Senckenberg Natural History Collections Dresden, A.B. Meyer Building, 01109 Dresden, Germany Hessisches Landesmuseum Darmstadt, Friedensplatz 1, 64283 Darmstadt, Germany Zoologische Staatssammlung Miinchen (ZSM-SNSB), Miinchhausenstr. 21, 81247 Miinchen, Germany Senckenberg Gesellschaft fiir Naturforschung, Senckenberganlage 25, 60325 Frankfurt/Main, Germany DN oR WN http://zoobank. org/F50B443E-265E-4C93-S8B7D-S55F793075C1F Corresponding author: Mark-Oliver Rodel (mo.roedel@mfn.berlin) Academic editor: Rafe Brown # Received 2 August 2019 Accepted 11 June 2020 Published 1 September 2020 Abstract Zoologists have widely acknowledged the utility of classification systems for characterising variation in anuran egg and clutch types, tadpole morphotypes, embryonic and tadpole development, amplexus types and reproductive modes. These classification systems have facilitated unambiguous communication between researchers, often working in completely different fields (e.g. taxonomy, ecol- ogy, behaviour), as well as comparisons among studies. A syntactic system, classifying anuran call guilds, is so far lacking. Based on examination of the calls of 1253 anuran species we present a simple, easy to use dichotomous key and guild system for classifying anuran advertisement calls — the call type most frequently emitted by anurans and studied by researchers. The use of only three call elements, namely clearly-defined calls, notes, and pulses, plus presence or absence of frequency modulation, allows assigning all cur- rently known anuran advertisement calls to one of eight distinct call guilds defined here. This novel toolkit will facilitate comparative studies across the many thousand anuran species, and may help to unravel drivers of anuran call evolution, and to identify ecological patterns at the level of acoustic communities. Key Words Amphibia, Anura, bioacoustics, call guilds Introduction or evolutionary research questions (Ord et al. 2013; Sch- iest! and Johnson 2013), and also used data from such Communication strategies are omnipresent across all forms of life, ranging from prokaryotes, to plants, fun- gi, and animals (Miller and Bassler 2001; Bradbury and Vehrencamp 2011). The reasons why organisms commu- nicate with each other are, thus, manifold. Essential ele- ments in all forms of communication are (1) a sender and a receiver, and (2) a signal of sufficient detectability and distinctiveness, so as to avoid loss of information or mis- interpretation by the receiver (Torricelli et al. 1986; Lu- cass et al. 2016). Researchers from many disciplines have exploited communication signals to address behavioural, signals in integrative taxonomic approaches to the study of various animal groups, including anuran amphibians (Padial et al. 2010; Kohler et al. 2017). The form of communication most frequently used by anurans 1s acoustic (Gerhardt and Huber 2002; for an in- depth review see Kohler et al. 2017). This form of com- munication co-evolved presumably along with hearing, allowing for precise sender—receiver communication sys- tems to evolve (e.g. Tembrock 1982; Ryan 2001; Desut- ter-Grandcolas 2002; Gerhardt and Huber 2002). Acoustic signals have the potential to cover a broad spatial range, to Copyright Mike Emmrich et al. This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY 4.0), which per- mits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited. 516 be characterized by rapid signal transfer rates, and to con- vey directionality (.e., the location of the sender may be identifiable to the receiver; Rothganger and Rothganger 2011). Acoustic signals are usually generated by oscillation of internal (birds, mammals, amphibians, fish), or external morphological structures (e.g. insects) (Gerhardt and Huber 2002; Bradbury and Vehrencamp 2011). These signals are mostly transmitted by air but also by using other material as carrier substrates (e.g. water or soil; Yager 1992; Platz 1993; Christensen-Dalsgaard and Elepfandt 1995; Seidel 1999; Lewis et al. 2001; Seidel et al. 2001; Bradbury and Vehren- camp 2011; Irisarri et al. 2011; Zheng et al. 2011). The main functions of intra-specific acoustic communication are at- traction, detection and selection of mates, territoriality, and / or exchange of other information (e.g. warning or release; Bee and Gerhardt 2002; Ballentine et al. 2004; Wollenberg and Harvey 2010; Stephan and Zuberbuhler 2014). A considerable diversity of acoustic mating signals ex- ists in anuran amphibians, and because they are the prima- ry mate-recognition signals, they are usually species-spe- cific (Ryan 2001; Gerhardt and Huber 2002; Kohler et al. 2017). Anuran call patterns are assumed to be largely ge- netically determined (Gerhardt et al. 1980; Duellman and Trueb 1994; Hédl 1996; Hoskin et al. 2005), with limit- ed variation among individuals and populations (but see discussion of intra-specific call variation in Wells 2007 and Kohler et al. 2017). This particularly concerns the so- called advertisement calls, used (mainly) by males to ad- vertise their location and to attract females (Mecham 1960; Zweifel 1968; Forester 1973; Ryan 2001; Gerhardt and Huber 2002; Wycherley et al. 2002; McLean et al. 2013). In addition to understanding their function in mate at- traction, taxonomists have made use of the species-specific and highly stereotyped nature of advertisement calls (Blair 1955, 1958; Littlejohn 1959; Schiotz 1964, 1967, 1971, 1973), and of their simple characteristics, to identify and delimit frog species (see Kohler et al. 2017 for review). However, gaps in our understanding persist, and lead to questions over which factors drive the evolution of calls and trigger the differences in advertisement calls among species. Some properties of anuran advertisement calls are impacted by morphology. For example, frequency-related call characters usually correlate with body size (Ryan 2001; Gerhardt and Huber 2002). Although some call parameters can be modified by physiology (e.g., a frog’s hormonal state; Wilczynski and Chu 2001), or temperature (Gerhardt 1978), the main bioacoustic characteristics of anuran calls are interpreted as fully heritable and only in exceptional cases shaped by learning (Dawson and Ryan 2009). From an evolutionary perspective, anuran advertisement calls are thus controlled by selection: sexual selection is most often discussed (Bradbury and Vehrencamp 2011), but an addi- tional, less frequently-explored component is natural se- lection, due to the abiotic and biotic environment through which acoustic signals are transmitted (Marten and Maler 1977; Wiley and Richards 1978; Bullen and Fricke 1982; Forrest et al. 1992; Gerhardt and Huber 2002; Swearingen and White 2007; Bradbury and Vehrencamp 2011). zse.pensoft.net Emmrich, M. et al.: Anuran call guilds Anuran calls are structurally very variable (Heyer and Reid 2003; Kohler et al. 2017), leading to a broad range of definitions of call structures, complicating their com- parability (Thompson et al. 1994; Gerhardt 1998; Ragge and Reynolds 1998; Gerhardt and Huber 2002; Kohler et al. 2017). A unified syntactic (1.e. structural) classification system, complementing available and widely applied se- mantic (i.e. functional) classification systems (compare Tembrock 1982; Gerhardt and Huber 2002) would there- fore be desirable to facilitate communication among vari- ous sub-disciplines, eliminate imprecise terminology, and reduce ambiguity in interactions among researchers from different backgrounds or disciplines (Littlejohn 2001). Semantic classification systems for anuran calls were proposed by Bogart (1960), Littlejohn (1977), and Wells (1977), and recently reviewed and updated by Toledo et al. (2015). On the other hand, Littleyohn (2001) suggested a syntactic classification. While semantic categorisations are needed to understand a species’ behaviour and com- munication relative to conspecifics and the environment (Wells 1977; Toledo et al. 2015), the content of such in- formation 1s not unique to a species. Syntactic classifica- tions in contrast, focus on the structure of calls, and tradi- tionally have been preferred by taxonomists (Thompson et al. 1994; Ragge and Reynolds 1998). A commonly used structural approach 1s guild classification. It can be particularly useful in understanding complex patterns in evolution and ecology (Wiens 1989; Williams and Hero 1998). Classifying advertisement calls into structural classes, or guilds, could, for instance, help to improve the understanding of complex interspecific soundscapes, or provide objective means of characterising acoustic par- titioning of diverse species communities (Morton 1975; Hansen 1979; Rothstein and Fleischer 1987). The use of an objective, purely structural classification system could also allow for neutral baselines in the development of a hypothesis-driven framework to test predictions con- cerning natural versus sexual selection in call evolution. Herein, we follow a syntactic, guild-based approach in describing anuran advertisement calls. We follow the definition of advertisement calls by Toledo et al. (2015): a call produced in the “breeding season to attract mates and to segregate calling individuals”. For other call types and their definitions see Wells (1977), Toledo et al. (2015) and Kohler et al. (2017). Our guild classification is based on the analysis of calls of 1253 anuran species from around the globe (Suppl. material 1: Table S1). The aim of this paper is to propose a guild classifica- tion based on the acoustic properties of anuran advertise- ment calls. We do this by further developing the syntactic approach suggested by Littlejohn (2001). We focus on ad- vertisement calls, because: (1) they are the most frequent- ly used call type in taxonomic work, (2) they are the most commonly emitted call of frogs and therefore easy to collect and most accessible for analyses, and (3) they are species-specific because they are the primary mate-recog- nition signal in anuran amphibians. Thus advertisement calls should be under strong selection. A syntactic guild Zoosyst. Evol. 96 (2) 2020, 515-525 classification for anuran advertisement calls should there- fore facilitate addressing evolutionary and functional as- pects in studies on amphibian biology and ecology. Material and methods Data collection Herein, we aim to simplify and unify syntactic definitions of advertisement call characters in a way such that they can unambiguously be applied to mate-recognition acous- tic signals of all anuran species. To establish an overview of advertisement call variability, we compiled and anal- ysed advertisement calls from anuran species from around the globe. For these baseline data, we used call collections (Suppl. material 1: References), databases (Suppl. materi- al 1: Web sources for calls used in this study), published call descriptions (Suppl. material 1: References), as well as our own call recordings of species from Africa, Mad- agascar and Guyana, in an attempt to cover a geographi- cally and phylogenetically wide range of anuran diversity. Because our primary goal was to include as much glob- al frog diversity as possible, we did not apply any stand- ardised search procedures (e.g., key word searches in Web of Science or Google Scholar), but simply accessed calls from freely available call collections, our own sound li- braries, and published taxonomic papers. We used call descriptions that were published primarily after 1990, be- cause earlier publications contained limited acoustic infor- mation, due to former technical limitations. To get com- parable recordings, we re-sampled available recordings to uncompressed wav-format, with a sampling rate of 44.1 kHz. We used the software Soundruler 0.9.6 (Gridi-Papp 2007) to measure call variables and the software package Seewave (Sueur et al. 2008) for R to visualise waveforms and frequency spectra (R Core Team 2013). Oscillograms (waveforms) and audio spectrograms as well as results of the Fast Fourier Transformation (FFT; frequency spec- trum) were examined for temporal and spectral charac- ters, respectively (using 44.1 kHz sample ratio, 16 bits resolution, FFT window width = 256, window function = Hanning”). The chosen FFT width represented the best compromise to achieve usable resolution and informative visualisation at both the temporal and spectral domain (Kohler et al. 2017). In cases where we had only published data available, but not the original recording, we incor- porated the published data in our data set. Classifying a call as an advertisement call usually followed the original assignments of a call description by the respective authors, but we verified these assignments against the advertise- ment call definition by Toledo et al. (2015) before adding a call to our database (Suppl. material 1). In total, we gathered published calls, call descriptions, or original recordings for 1426 species from 230 genera and 43 families. Anuran nomenclature and taxonomy were obtained directly from databases and publications (Frost 2019). For further background information and 517 best practices advice, concerning call recording, analyses, interpretation and presentation, see Kohler et al. (2017). Definitions of anuran advertisement call units For the purpose of developing a syntactic classification system of anuran advertisement call diversity, we estab- lished a globally applicable scheme that is as simple as possible but still sufficiently detailed to cover the current- ly known range of variation in these acoustic signals. To this aim, we surveyed advertisement calls of all includ- ed species, comprehensively, striving to identify distinct structural elements matching the criteria. We incorporated these elements into a dichotomous key, to allow for objec- tive assignment of any species’ advertisement call to a dis- tinct call guild. Following previous definitions of acoustic units of structural signal variation (Kohler et al. 2017), we identified three basic elements in common, apparently sufficient to comprehensively characterise acoustic signal variability: the call, notes, and pulses. As the definitions of calls and call series 1s ambigu- ously dealt with in the literature (Kohler et al. 2017), it has previously been problematic to articulate an unequiv- ocal, universal definition of an anuran “call”. However, such a definition is crucial, because a lack of consistency among disciplines and individual researchers in terminol- ogy related to a species’ call, a call series, and note, often hampers the interpretation and understanding of call de- scriptions. Previously such standardisations were largely ignored, resulting in idiosyncratic call descriptions that do not allow for comparative or meta-analyses. To minimise these problems, we followed and refined the note-centred approach suggested by Kohler et al. (2017) to define fun- damental units of advertisement call variation. The term call is here used synonymous with advertise- ment call — the functional signal for mate-recognition, as the main acoustic unit in frog vocalisation. Calls are separated from other calls by silent inter-call intervals, typically longer (often several times longer) than the call itself. A call series is the temporal repetition of identical calls, repeated at rather regular intervals, and separat- ed by larger gaps of silence from other call series (note that the definitions of our advertisement call guilds below do not take into account whether calls are arranged in series or not; only the call unit itself was considered). Under this definition, a call may be comprised of one or more subunits (Fig. 1b-e). These may differ in length and structure and are classified as either notes or pulses. Calls are often sub-structured into two or more notes. Notes are subunits separated by intervals of silence (100% amplitude modulation), with the duration of these intervals being usually short relative to the duration of the note. Peri- ods of silence are longer between notes than between pulses (see below) that form such notes (if pulsed), and shorter than the periods of silence between calls. zse.pensoft.net THA HH-HH-HH ‘HH: HH-HH- HHH TATA Time Figure 1. Basic types of anuran vocalizations based on their temporal structure, shown as schematic waveforms, modified after Littlejohn (2001): (a) non-pulsed call, (b) pulsed call, (c) call with uniform pulsed notes, (d) complex call containing different note types, and (e) two complex calls in a call series. Black arrows mark inter-note intervals and red arrow marks inter-call interval. It must be mentioned that in a call consisting of only one unit (with or without subunits of the pulse category; Fig. 1a, b), the definition of a note and a call would apply simultaneously to the same, making them synonymous (Kohler et al. 2017). Because we follow this note-centred approach, we use the term call for the broader, encompass- ing unit. With this definition, 1t is only necessary to use all three terms if a call consists of at least three different units, separated by unequal periods of silence (Fig. 1c—e). Notes sometimes contain a sub-structure produced by amplitude modulation within the note. These units are de- fined as pulses, following Kohler et al. (2017): A pulse is the shortest, undividable unit in anuran vo- calisation, with a typical duration of less than 50 ms. In addition to these formalised syntactical advertise- ment call units, we included modulation of dominant spectral frequency. Frequency modulation can be depicted visually from spectrograms by a clearly visible increase or decrease in dominant frequency (“dfrq” in Hz). Mod- ulation can be objectively quantified by subtracting the end-frequency from the start-frequency, and dividing this value by call duration (in ms). We calculated dominant frequency modulation (dfrq/ms) of all species with calls exhibiting frequency modulation, based on one represent- ative call of the respective species. Values of > 1/-1 Hz/ Key to anuran Advertisement Call Guilds (compare Fig. 2 and descriptions below) 1 call consists of only one acoustic Unit .................cceceeeeeee 1’ call contains several acoustic UNITS ....... cece eee ee ms were considered as a significant change in dominant frequency and all species were assigned to one of two bi- nary character states, namely “not frequency modulated” (< 1/-1 Hz/ms) or “frequency modulated” (> 1/-1 Hz/ms). After filtering our initial recordings representative of 1426 species and removing calls of insufficient quality (e.g. call descriptions lacking data for frequency modula- tion; visualisation only comprising either oscillogram or spectrogram; figures of insufficient quality; or recordings consisting of only one single call) from our dataset, we retained calls of 1253 species for final classification (Sup- pl. material 1: Table S1). We used these remaining calls to formalise the advertisement call structural (syntactic) guild classification presented in the following key. Results The combination of call units (call, note, and pulse) and frequency modulation allowed us to define eight distinct structural (syntactic) call guilds (Guilds A—H, Table 1). With a dichotomous key, the advertisement calls of each of the 1253 species could readily be assigned to one such guild. Below, we provide the guild classification key (Fig. 2), we summarise call guilds, and provide illustrative ex- amples (respective spectrograms and waveforms, plus species identifications in Fig. 3.) 2 dominant frequency without significant change over Call CUraTiON................cccccceseeecceeceeesencesecencoessecsseceeecesceeseereeesseceres A ee oer Aaa eB rN cen Fh ol SY IR 2» Any ek ae PIT si Call Guild A: “non-frequency modulated, non-pulsed simple call” 2’ dominant frequency with significant change over call Curation ................ccccecsseceseceecoesceccsecesccescoussecceecaseceesenscecseneeseeees zse.pensoft.net Call Guild B: “frequency modulated, non-pulsed simple call” Zoosyst. Evol. 96 (2) 2020, 515-525 519 S Call-COMpPuiSes- OC Ses Ut! POMTOISS:. 252 eir AN Pes Jere esl hee TA Ake noe PANN ce ee Sed ae es Bek ie en a Bn Bek ee Sees BL 4 Siig REA COMM SSS: Se Veal MOM SSC INGLES = a: a aiecebuhtes’ dav rues cerscnars bad etnbe a2 pw assests bitbend Ae eynstnie ipa ee tech Os AA ates ranetnarchh tht dt i wrecaneauennirta 5 4 = dominant frequency without significant change over call duration:...... Call Guild C: “non-frequency modulated pulsed call” 4’ ~~ dominant frequency with significant change over call duration: ............ Call Guild D: “frequency modulated pulsed call” 5 call-camprises several structurally-cmorée-or less) -sSiimi(lat NOTES -9. «slate alee caleetiles #004 cee lde guncesdleble ve Uibdlowid Sade bade etina Pant ldiee 6 5” “callecomipenses-siruetuirallycish Methy CIMTEKENT NOTES), «cet faane segndoocsiss cheng tans bnadl tps ais ve esch tering anie ABbgpeais cicheea afer sme ape dels veceeees 7 6 cominantdrequency Wwiinout- significant change -ever-call CMFATIOR: rect ces) Aa leas 2... SOMOS, Cece, PPO cea) PR ees 22s canbe a a itd pst ne eeaeteg ie © Hee #8 hart BR ea SRE Latent oben esate vee Call Guild E: “non-frequency modulated call with uniform notes” G.” “eminent irequency-Withssisrificant-ctriaiige: Over Call=CVatiOne nhs ses a. beara dau ata icceemadealsnseetarang ede stn apes .... Call Guild F: “frequency modulated call with uniform notes” 7 dominant frequency without significant change over Call CUIraAtiON...........cccccecc ccc ecec eee ece eee ees eeeeeeeeeee eee eeseeeeeeeeeaseeaeeeeeeees dytediateceatd Call Guild G: “non-frequency modulated complex cal ji 7’ dominant frequency with significant change over call duration in at least one of the distinct note typeS.............ccceeeeeee Short description of call guilds with species examples Here we summarise the “diagnostic” characters of the dif- ferent call guilds, give some species examples for each guild and refer to respective illustrations of selected ex- emplary calls (Fig. 3). The sources for the specific exam- ples are summarised in Suppl. material 1: Table S1. Call Guild A “non-frequency modulated, non-pulsed simple call”: call consists of one single continuous signal (which can be of any duration) with no significant change in dominant frequency. Examples: Alytes cisternasii (Alytidae), Bombina bombina (Bombinatoridae), E/euth- erodactylus tonyi (Eleutherodactylidae), Heleophryne de- pressa (Heleophrynidae), Rana arvalis (Ranidae). Call Guild B “frequency modulated, non-pulsed sim- ple call”: call consists of one single continuous signal Call Mn dh’ Behe TRON Call Guild H: “frequency modulated complex call” (which can be of any duration) with a significant change in dominant frequency. Examples: Rhaebo haematiti- cus (Bufonidae), Pristimantis bambu (Craugastoridae), Ameerega pepperi (Dendrobatidae), Kassina senegalen- sis (Hyperoliidae), Leptodactylus fuscus (Leptodactyl- idae), Limnodynastes peronii (Limnodynastidae), Aus- trochaperina fryi (Microhylidae), Strongylopus grayii (Pyxicephalidae), Chiromantis vittiger (Rhacophoridae). Call Guild C: “non-frequency modulated, pulsed call”: call comprised of several similar, but distinguishable acous- tic signals (pulses). Pulses are arranged in a single group (note = call), meaning that intervals between pulses are equally long, but much shorter than inter-call intervals. The dominant frequency does not change over the call duration. Examples: Dendropsophus tritaeniatus (Hylidae), Eleuth- erodactylus toa (Eleutherodactylidae), Hemisus marmora- tus (Hemisotidae), Cophixalus concinnus (Microhylidae). een single multi-unit one pulsed note multi-note ff . x / Ke non- non- non- non- modulated modulated modulated modulated modulated modulated modulated modulated wee oonooa —— $BGSSS Oooanmc i BX BH Mi canal i 8 Bt Sooo eu # AI iiiititins ition Guild A Guild B Guild C Guild D HH HH tA Guild E Guild F Guild G Guild H Figure 2. Key to anuran advertisement call guilds (compare text); each guild illustrated by schematic waveform and spectrogram. zse.pensoft.net 520 Emmrich, M. et al.: Anuran call guilds "Guild A Guild B Qo— & Guild c "| Guild D | i | nee UPPED DEP DD DD N ! t x > oO od o — oO © LL T ne EE EE B cuild € Guild F r pei eh vittp ee XD HDHD. te Rast ob. 8— Guild G Guild H fi at | i 1 0 1 Duration [s] wr Figure 3. Examples for all different anuran advertisement call guilds (A—H), with a time scale of 0 to 1 s on x-axis and frequency scale of 0 to 8 kHz on y-axis (compare text). Guild A) non-frequency modulated, non-pulsed simple call (Bombina bombina; Bom- binatoridae; dfrq/ms = 0.00 Hz/ms) (based on Schneider 2005); Guild B) frequency modulated, non-pulsed simple call (Leptodacty- lus fuscus, Leptodactylidae; dfrq/ms = 7.22 Hz/ms) (based on Marquez et al. 2002); Guild C) non-frequency modulated pulsed call (Hyla meridionalis;, Hylidae; dfrq/ms = 0.67 Hz/ms) (based on Maso and Pijoan 2011); Guild D) frequency modulated pulsed call (Hyperolius pickersgilli,; Hyperoliidae; dfrq/m = 2.32 Hz/ms) (based on Du Preez and Carruthers 2009); Guild E) non-frequency modulated call with uniform notes (Sclerophrys mauritanica;, Bufonidae; dfrq/ms = 0.31 Hz/ms) (based on Maso and Pijoan 2011); Guild F) frequency modulated call, with uniform notes (Pseudopaludicola boliviana, Leptodactylidae; dfrq/ms = 2.38 Hz/ms) (based on Marquez et al. 2002); Guild G) non-frequency modulated complex call (Smilisca sila, Hylidae; dfrq/m = 0.43 Hz/ms) (based on Ibanéz 1999); Guild H) frequency modulated complex call (Hyperolius nasutus; Hyperoliidae; dfrq/ms = 1.66 Hz/ms) (based on Du Preez and Carruthers 2009). zse.pensoft.net Zoosyst. Evol. 96 (2) 2020, 515-525 Table 1. Number of species studied (N = 1253) per call guild; for guild definitions compare text and Fig. 2. N Percentage Guild A 130 10.4% Guild B 198 15.8% Guild C 454 36.3% Guild D 247 19.7% Guild E 81 6.5% Guild F 22 1.8% Guild G 93 7AN Guild H 28 2.3% Total 1253 100.0% Call Guild D: “frequency modulated, pulsed call”: call comprised of several similarly structured, but distin- guishable acoustic signals (pulses). Pulses are arranged in a single group meaning that intervals between puls- es are equally long, but much shorter than inter-call in- tervals. The dominant frequency changes significantly over the call duration. Examples: Pristimantis w-nigrum (Craugastoridae), Acris gryllus (Hylidae), Diasporus vocator (Eleutherodactylidae), Mantidactylus tricinctus (Mantellidae), Agalychnis callidryas (Phyllomedusidae), Ptychadena taenioscelis (Ptychadenidae), Rana muscosa (Ranidae), Kurixalus appendiculatus (Rhacophoridae). Call Guild E “non-frequency modulated call with uni- form notes”: call comprised of several similarly struc- tured notes. Dominant frequency does not change over the call duration. Examples: Boana leptolineata (Hylidae), Blommersia grandisonae (Mantellidae), Leptobrachium leucops (Megophryidae), Oreophryne clamata (Micro- hylidae), Mixophyes fleayi (Myobatrachidae), Lithobates virgatipes (Ranidae). Call Guild F “frequency modulated call with uniform notes”: call comprised of several distinguishable notes. These notes are structurally very similar to each other. Dom- inant frequency changes significantly over the call duration. Examples: Boana almendarizae, Osteocephalus yasuni, Sci- nax nebulosus (Hylidae), Pseudopaludicola boliviana (Lep- todactylidae), Ptychadena schillukorum (Ptychadenidae). Call Guild G “non-frequency modulated, complex call”: call comprised of several notes. The signals are ar- ranged in several, at least two, structurally different note types. Dominant frequency does not change over the call duration. Examples: Melanophryniscus atroluteus (Bufo- nidae), Hyperolius benguellensis (Hyperoliidae), Boophis bottae (Mantellidae), Litoria fallax (Pelodryadidae). Call Guild H “frequency modulated, complex call”: call comprised of several notes, at least two being structurally different. Dominant frequency changes significantly over the call duration in at least one of the distinct note types, e.g. Smilisca sila (Hylidae), Cochranella granulosa (Cen- trolenidae), Engystomops pustulosus (Leptodactylidae). Discussion Syntactic classification systems allow for unambiguous communication between researchers and comparisons be- 521 tween studies. For instance, the frequent adoption, appli- cation, and widespread use of clear definitions of anuran egg types, tadpole morphotypes, and developmental stages (Gosner 1960; Altig and Johnston 1989; Altig and McDiar- mid 2007; Schulze et al. 2015), anuran reproduction modes (Duellman and Trueb 1994; Haddad and Prado 2005), modes of amplexus (Carvayal-Casto et al. 2020), and an- uran call types (Wells 1977; Toledo et al. 2015), have demonstrated their substantial value for herpetologists. In this paper, we present a simplified guild classifi- cation system for anuran advertisement calls. In order to avoid assignment of individuals from one species to different guilds, we have not included body size-driven traits like dominant frequency, temperature-driven traits like pulse rate, or motivation-driven traits like inter-call interval duration or call rate. The use of only three call el- ements (call, note, pulse, plus the presence or absence of frequency modulation), allows for the unambiguous allo- cation of any anuran advertisement call currently known to us to a distinct syntactic, non-functional, call guild. The number of species investigated by us, although covering the majority of families, habitats and regions, represents only a small proportion (about 17%) of the currently known anuran species (> 7100; Frost 2019; last accessed 10 March 2020). It is thus possible, or even like- ly, that advertisement calls discovered in the future may not perfectly fit our proposed guild system. However, this system could easily be extended. For example, the proposed guilds could be divided by differentiating with- in a guild the maximum of amplitude (initial, centred or terminal of call), the dominant frequency (low, medium or high-pitched calls), the direction of frequency modula- tion (negative or positive), or by the distinction between pulsatile and tonal calls. Our definition of call guilds may not only allow for better comparisons between variable call descriptions, as suggested by Krause (1987), but a syntactic classification based on acoustics may also enable improved insight into the life-history of species. Most anurans behaviourally select and call from species-specific breeding sites (Du- ellman and Trueb 1994; Wells 2007). Different calling behaviour and different habitats both affect signal prop- agation through call-specific environments in different ways. Abiotic (e.g., humidity or air pressure) and biotic factors (e.g., vegetation density or structure) may affect sound waves (Bradbury and Vehrencamp 2011). Propaga- tion properties may change due to an individual (usually male) frog, calling from different substrates or microhab- itats, such as trees, leaf-litter, from water, underground, or, if they call singly or in a chorus (Lopez et al. 1988; Forrest 1994; Lardner and bin Lakim 2002). Intrinsic fac- tors such as body condition or fatigue can also change propagation properties (Humfeld 2013; Jansen et al. 2016). Call properties and calling behaviour, thus, may be adjusted behaviourally to avoid or reduce information loss or alteration of structure. Many breeding sites share similar features, such as stagnant versus flowing streams, open or closed vegetation (Hédl 1977; Kwet 2001; zse.pensoft.net 522 Schliiter 2005), and characteristics of such microhabitats may alter sound in specific ways. For instance, torrent water introduces background noise, which may simply mask the call (or certain frequency components); dense vegetation may cause scattering, blurring, or reflections, particularly of amplitude-modulated calls (Bradbury and Vehrencamp 2011). Thus, habitat characteristics may force or select for species living in similar environments to share specific combinations of call elements. It is pos- sible that the call guild types presented herein are coupled to certain behavioural patterns and thus allow predictions about the behaviour of species. Several correlations be- tween bioacoustics and habitat, behaviour, or morphol- ogy have already become known in frogs, as well as in other animals (Etges 1987; Krause 1987; Hodl 1996; Martins et al. 2006; Vasconcelos and Rossa-Feres 2008; Both and Grand 2012; Sinsch et al. 2012). For example, Neotropical birds living in dense understorey show less frequency modulations than birds of more open habitats (Morton 1975). However, it is unlikely that call proper- ties are determined by single factors; acoustic signals will be, apart from phylogeny, shaped by various environmen- tal and species-specific characters (Goutte et al. 2018). By classifying calls according to structural elements, our syntactic call guilds might assist in detecting such general correlations between call characteristics and nat- ural history, or habitat. Based on the calls of the 1253 Species considered in this study, the most common call guilds were guilds C and D, followed by guilds A and B (Table 1). Calls with more complex structures, like guilds E to H, were less frequent. Frequency modulation was most often associated with simple and relatively short calls, whereas complex calls were less likely to consist of frequency modulated elements. Our guild system is not suited to discriminate between closely related species or to describe species; it is, thus, not a tool for taxonomy. However, it might be used as a first “sorting step” for an acoustic characterisation in call descriptions, and it may help to facilitate the understand- ing of anuran advertisement call evolution. For example, a semantic classification of advertisement calls assumes that similar calls comprise the same information (Wells 1977; Toledo et al. 2015). However, the informational content of advertisement calls often differs (Ryan 2001; Gerhardt and Huber 2002). An increase in call complexi- ty may be related to more, or different, social interactions, like in Engystomops pustulosus (Ryan et al. 1982; Ryan 1985; Ryan and Rand 1990; Baugh and Ryan 2010). The ultimate adaptive significance for such reduction or addi- tion of information may be indicative of interactions of species with their abiotic and biotic environments. The simple classification of call guilds based on structure may facilitate interpretation of these acoustic interactions and help clarify the origin of call components or structures. Finally, we envision that our syntactic call guide classi- fication scheme will be a useful tool set for future me- ta-analyses and comparative studies concerning the evo- lution of anuran acoustic signals. However, inclusion of zse.pensoft.net Emmrich, M. et al.: Anuran call guilds questions relating to how the environment, morphology, life-history and phylogeny shape anuran advertisement calls, remains a challenge for forthcoming studies. Acknowledgments We thank Franco Andreone, Roberto Alonso, Rainer Gunther, Axel Kwet, Rafael Marquez and Gongalo M. Rosa for providing sound recordings, Ariel Rodriguez for fruitful collaboration and productive discussions, the ed- itor Rafe Brown and two anonymous reviewers for their constructive critique and suggestions. References Altig R, Johnston GF (1989) Guilds of anuran larvae: relationship among developmental modes, morphologies and habitats. Herpe- tological Monographs 3: 81—109. https://doi.org/10.2307/1466987 Altig R, McDiarmid RW (2007) Morphological diversity and evolution of egg and clutch structure in amphibians. Herpetological Mono- graphs 21: 1-32. https://doi.org/10.1655/06-005. 1 Ballentine B, Hyman J, Nowicki S (2004) Vocal performance influences female response to male bird song: an experimental test. Behavioral Ecology 15: 163-168. https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/arg090 Baugh AT, Ryan MJ (2010) The relative value of call embellishment in tungara frogs. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 65: 359-367. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-010-1053-6 Bee MA, Gerhardt HC (2002) Individual voice recognition in a territori- al frog (Rana catesbeiana). Proceedings of the Royal Society B 269: 1443-1448. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2002.2041 Blair WF (1955) Mating call and stage of speciation in the Microhyla olivacea —M. carolinensis complex. Evolution 9: 469-480. https:// doi.org/10.1111/).1558-5646.1955 .tb01556.x Blair WF (1958) Mating call and stages of speciation of anuran amphibi- ans. American Naturalist 92: 27-51. https://doi.org/10.1086/282007 Bogart CM (1960) The influence of sound on the behavior of amphib- ians and reptiles. In: Lanyon ED, Tvolga WN (Eds) Animal sounds and communication. American Institute of Biological Science, Washington, 137-320. Both C, Grant T (2012) Biological invasions and the acoustic niche: the effect of bullfrog calls on the acoustic signals of white-band- ed tree frogs. Biology Letters 8: 714-716. https://doi.org/10.1098/ rsbl.2012.0412 Bradbury JW, Vehrencamp SL (2011) Principles of animal communica- tion. Sinauer Associates, Sunderland, 697 pp. Bullen R, Fricke F (1982) Sound propagation through vegetation. Jour- nal of Sound and Vibration 80: 11—23. https://doi.org/10.1016/0022- 460X(82)90387-X Carvajal-Castro JD, Lopez-Aguirre Y, Ospina-L AM, Santos JC, Roja B, Vargas-Salinas F (2020) Much more than a clasp: evolutionary patterns of amplexus diversity in anurans. Biological Journal of the Linnean Society 129: 652-663. https://doi.org/10.1093/biolinnean/ blaa009 Christensen-Dalsgaard J, Elepfandt A (1995) Biophysics of underwater hearing in the clawed frog, Xenopus laevis. Journal of Comparative Physiology A 176: 317-324. https://do1.org/10.1007/BF00219057 Zoosyst. Evol. 96 (2) 2020, 515-525 Dawson B, Ryan MJ (2009) Early experience leads to changes in the advertisement calls of male Physalaemus pustulosus. Copeia 2009: 221-226. https://doi.org/10.1643/CE-07-254 Desutter-Grandcolas L (2002) Phylogeny and evolution of acoustic com- munication in extant Ensifera (Insecta, Orthoptera). Zoologica Scrip- ta 32: 525-561 https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1463-6409.2003.00142.x. Duellman WE, Trueb L (1994) Biology of amphibians. The Johns Hop- kins University Press, Baltimore, 670 pp. Du Preez L, Carruthers V (2009) A complete guide to the frogs of southern Africa. Struik Nature, Cape Town, South Africa, 488 pp. + audio CD. Etges WJ (1987) Call site choice in male anurans. Copeia 1987: 910- 923. https://doi.org/10.2307/1445554 Forester DC (1973) Mating call as a reproductive isolating mechanism between Scaphiopus bombifrons and S. hammondii. Copeia 1973: 60-67. https://doi.org/10.2307/1442358 Forrest TG (1994) From sender to receiver: Propagation and environ- mental effects on acoustic signals. American Zoologist 34: 644-654. https://do1.org/10.1093/icb/34.6.644 Forrest TG, Miller GL, Zagar JR (1992) Sound propagation in shallow water: implications for acoustic communication by aquatic animals. Bioacoustics 4: 259-270. https://doi.org/10.1080/09524622.1993.1 0510437 Frost DR (2019) Amphibian species of the world: an online reference. Version 6.0. American Museum of Natural History, New York, USA. http://research.amnh.org/herpetology/amphibia/index.htm [last ac- cessed on 10 March 2020] Gerhardt HC (1978) Temperature coupling in the vocal communication system of the gray tree frog, Hyla versicolor. Science 199: 992-994. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.199.4332.992 Gerhardt HC, Guttman SI, Karlin AA (1980) Natural hybrids between Hyla cinerea and Hyla gratiosa: Morphology, vocalization and electropho- retic analysis. Copeia 1980: 577-584. https://doi.org/10.2307/1444432 Gerhardt CH, Huber F (2002) Acoustic communication in insects and anurans. The University of Chicago Press, Chicago, 542 pp. Gosner KL (1960) A simplified table staging anuran embryo and larvae with notes on identification. Herpetologica 16: 183-190. Goutte S, Dubois A, Howard SD, Marquez Rowley JJL, Dehling JM, Grandcolas P, Xiong RC, Legendre F (2018) How the environment shapes animal signals: a test of the acoustic adaptation hypothesis in frogs. Journal of Evolutionary Biology 31: 148-158. https://doi. org/10.1111/jeb.13210 Gridi-Papp M (2007) SoundRuler: Acoustic analysis for research and teaching. http://soundruler.sourceforge.net Haddad CFB, Prado CPA (2005) Reproductive modes in frogs and their unexpected diversity in the Atlantic forest of Brazil. BioScience 55: 207-217. https://doi.org/10.1641/0006-3568(2005)055[0207:RMI- FAT ]2.0.CO;2 Hansen P (1979) Vocal learning: its role in adapting sound structure to long-distance propagation and a hypothesis on its evolution. Animal Behaviour 27: 1270-1271. https://doi.org/10.1016/0003- 3472(79)90073-3 Hodl W (1977) Call differences and calling site segregation in anuran species from Central Amazonian floating meadows. Oecologica 28: 351-363. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00345990 H6dl W (1996) Wie verstandigen sich Frosche? Stapfia 47: 53-70. Hoskin CJ, Higgie M, McDonald KR, Moritz C (2005) Reinforcement drives rapid allopatric speciation. Nature 437: 1353-1356. https:// doi.org/10.1038/nature04004 523 Humfeld SC (2013) Condition-dependent signaling and adoption of mating tactics in an amphibian with energetic displays. Behavioral Ecology 25: 520-530. https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/art024 Ibafiez RD, Rand S, Ryan MJ, Jaramillo CAA (1999) Vocalizaciones de ranas y sapos del Monumento Natural Barro Colorado, Parque Nacional Soberania y areas adyacentes. audio CD. Sony Music En- tertainment, Costa Rica. Irisarri I, Vences M, San Mauro D, Glaw F, Zardoya R (2011) Reversal to air-driven sound production revealed by a molecular phylogeny of tongueless frogs, family Pipidae. BMC Evolutionary Biology 11: 114. https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2148-11-114 Jansen M, Masurowa A, O’ Hara RB (2016) Temporal variation, duty cy- cle and absolute calling effort during sustained calling of Leptodac- tylus mystacinus (Anura: Leptodactylidae). Salamandra 52: 328-336. Kohler J, Jansen M, Rodriguez A, Kok PJR, Toledo LF, Emmrich M, Glaw F, Haddad CFB, Rodel MO, Vences M (2017) The use of bio- acoustics in anuran taxonomy: theory, terminology, methods and recommendations for best practice. Zootaxa 4251: 1-124. https:// doi.org/10.11646/zootaxa.4251.1.1 Krause BL (1987) The niche hypothesis: How animals taught us to dance and sing. Whole Earth Review 57: 1-6. Kwet A (2001) Frésche im brasilianischen Araukarienwald, Audio CD. Natur und Tier-Verlag, Minster, Germany, 220 pp. Lardner B, bin Lakim M (2002) Tree-hole frogs exploit resonance ef- fects. Nature 420: 475-475. https://doi.org/10.1038/420475a Lewis ER, Narins PM, Cortopassi KA, Yamada WM, Poinar EH, Moore SW, Yu XL (2001) Do male white-lipped frogs use seismic signals for intraspecific communication? American Zoologist 41: 1185— 1199. https://doi.org/10.1093/icb/41.5.1185 Littlejohn MJ (1959) Call structure in a complex of seven species of Crinia (Anura, Leptodactylidae). Evolution 13: 452-468. https:// doi.org/10.2307/2406128 Littlejohn MJ (1977) Long-range acoustic communication in anurans: an integrated and evolutionary approach. In: Taylor DH, Guttman SI (Eds) The Reproductive Biology of Amphibians. Plenum, New York, 263-294. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4757-6781-0_8 Littlejohn MJ (2001) Patterns of differentiation in temporal properties of acoustic signals of anurans. In: Ryan MJ (Ed) Anuran communi- cation. Smithsonian Institute Press, Washington DC, 102-120. Lopez PT, Narins PM, Lewis ER, Moore SW (1988) Acoustically in- duced call modification in the white-lipped frog, Leptodactylus al- bilabris. Animal Behaviour 36: 1295-1308. https://doi.org/10.1016/ S0003-3472(88)80198-2 Lucass C, Eens M, Muller W (2016) When ambient noise impairs par- ent-offspring communication. Environmental Pollution 212: 592— 597. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.envpol.2016.03.015 Marquez R, De la Riva I, Bosch J, Matheu E (2002) Guia sonora de las ranas y sapos de Bolivia. Audio CD, alosa, sons de la natura, Barcelona, Spain. Marten K, Marler P (1977) Sound transmission and its significance for animal vocalization. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 2: 271— 290. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00299740 Martins IA, Almeida SC, Jim J (2006) Calling sites and acoustic par- titioning in species of Hyla nana and rubicundula groups (Anura, Hylidae). Herpetological Journal 16: 239-247, Maso A, Pijoan M (2011) Nuevas guias de campo anfibios y reptiles de la Peninsula Ibérica, Baleares y Canarias. Ediciones Omega, Barce- lona, Spain, 848 pp. + audio CD. zse.pensoft.net 524 McLean MJ, Bishop PJ, Nakagawa S (2013) Assessing the patterns of evolution in anuran vocal sexual signals. Evolutionary Biology 40: 141-149. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11692-012-9197-0 Mecham JS (1960) Introgressive hybridization between two southeastern treefrogs. Evolution 14: 445-457. https://doi.org/10.2307/2405994 Miller MB, Bassler BL (2001) Quorum sensing in bacteria. Annual Reviews of Microbiology 55: 165-199. https://doi.org/10.1146/an- nurev.micro.55.1.165 Morton ES (1975): Ecological sources of selection on avian sounds. The American Naturalist 109: 17-34. https://doi.org/10.1086/282971 Ord TJ, Stamps JA, Losos JB (2013) Convergent evolution in the territo- rial communication of a classic adaptive radiation: Caribbean Anolis lizards. Animal Behaviour 85: 1414—1426. https://doi.org/10.1016/). anbehav.2013.03.037 Padial JM, Miralles A, De la Riva I, Vences M (2010) The integra- tive future of taxonomy. Frontiers in Zoology 7: 16. https://doi. org/10.1186/1742-9994-7-16 Platz JE (1993) Rana subaquavocalis, a remarkable species of leop- ard frog (Rana pipiens complex) from southeastern Arizona that calls under water. Journal of Herpetology 27: 154-162. https://doi. org/10.2307/1564931 R Core Team (2013) R: A language and environment for statistical com- puting. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. http://www.R-project.org/ Ragge DR, Reynolds WJ (1998) The songs of grasshoppers and crickets of western Europe. Harley Books, Colchester, 596 pp. Rothganger H, Rothganger A (2011) Bioakustik I. Lehmanns Media, Berlin, 138 pp. Rothstein SI, Fleischer RC (1987) Vocal dialects and their possible rela- tion to honest status signaling in the Brown-headed Cowbird. Con- dor 89: 1—23. https://doi.org/10.2307/1368756 Ryan MJ (1985) The Tungara frog — a study in sexual selection and communication. University of Chicago Press, Chicago, 230 pp. Ryan MJ (2001) Anuran communication. Smithsonian Institution Press, Washington DC, 252 pp. Ryan MJ, Tuttle MD, Rand AS (1982) Bat predation and sexual ad- vertisement in a neotropical frog. The American Naturalist 119: 136-139. https://doi.org/10.1086/283899 Ryan MJ, Rand AS (1990) The sensory basis of sexual selection for complex calls in tungara frog, Physalaemus pustulosus (sexual se- lection for sensory exploitation). Evolution 44: 305-367. https://doi. org/10.2307/2409409 Schiestl FP, Johnson SD (2013) Pollinator-mediated evolution of floral signals. Trends in Ecology & Evolution 28: 307-315. https://doi. org/10.1016/j.tree.2013.01.019 Schiotz A (1964) The voices of some West African amphibians. Vi- denskabelige Meddelelser fra dansk Naturhistorik Forening 127: 35-83. Schiotz A (1967) The treefrogs (Rhacophoridae) of West Africa. Spolia Zoologica Musei Hauniensis 25: 1—346. Schiotz A (1971) Evolution and mating call: ecological consideration. Herpetological Review 3: 11. Schiotz A (1973) Evolution of anuran mating calls, ecological aspects. In: Vial JL (Ed) Evolutionary biology of the anurans, contemporary research on major problems. University of Missouri Press, Colum- bia, 311-319. Schlitter A (2005) Amphibien an einem Stillgewdsser in Peru. Edition Chimaira, Frankfurt/Main, 347 pp. zse.pensoft.net Emmrich, M. et al.: Anuran call guilds Schneider H (2005) Bioakustik der Froschlurche. Laurenti Verlag, Bielefeld, Germany, 136 pp. + audio CD. Schulze A, Jansen M, Kohler G (2015) Tadpole diversity of Bolivia’s lowland anuran communities: molecular identification, morpho- logical characterization, and ecological assignment. Zootaxa 4016: 1-111. https://doi.org/10.11646/zootaxa.4016.1.1 Seidel B (1999) Water-wave communication between territorial male Bombina variegata. Journal of Herpetology 33: 457-462. https:// doi.org/10.2307/1565643 Seidel B, Yamashita M, Choi IH, Dittami J (2001) Water wave commu- nication in the genus Bombina (Amphibia). Advances in Space Re- search 28: 589-594. https://do1.org/10.1016/S0273-1177(01)00386-6 Sinsch U, Lumkemann K, Rosar K, Schwarz C, Dehling JM (2012) Acoustic niche partitioning in an anuran community inhabiting an afromontane wetland (Butare, Rwanda). African Zoology 47: 60- 73. https://doi.org/10.1080/15627020.2012.11407524 Stephan C, Zuberbuhler K (2014) Predation affects alarm call usage in female Diana Monkeys (Cercopithecus diana diana). Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 68: 321-331. https://doi.org/10.1007/ s00265-013-1647-x Sueur J, Aubin T, Simonis C (2008) Seewave: a free modular tool for sound analysis and synthesis. Bioacoustics 18: 213-226. https://doi. org/10.1080/09524622.2008.9753600 Swearingen ME, White MJ (2007) Influence of scattering, atmospheric refraction, and ground effect on sound propagation through a pine forest. The Journal of Acoustic Society of America 122: 113-119. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.2735108 Tembrock G (1982) Tierstimmenforschung. A. Ziemsen Verlag, Witten- berg Lutherstadt, 240 pp. Thompson NS, LeDoux K, Moody K (1994) A system for describing bird song units. Bioacoustics 5: 267-279. https://doi.org/10. 1080/0 9524622.1994.9753257 Toledo LF, Martins IA, Bruschi DP, Passos MA, Alexandre C, Haddad CFB (2015) The anuran calling repertoire in the light of social context. Acta Ethologica 18: 87-99. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10211-014-0194-4 Torricelli P, Lugli M, Gandolfi G (1986) A quantitative analysis of the occurrence of visual and acoustic displays during the court- ship in the freshwater goby, Padogobius martensi (Gunther, 1961) (Pisces, Gobiidae). Bolletino di Zoologia 53: 85-89. https://doi. org/10.1080/11250008609355488 Vasconcelos TS, Rossa-Feres DC (2008) Habitat heterogeneity and use of physical and acoustic space in anuran communities in south eastern Brazil. Phyllomedusa 7: 127-142. https://doi.org/10.11606/ issn.2316-9079.v712p127-142 Wells KD (1977) The courtship of frogs. In: Taylor DH, Guttman SI (Eds) The Reproductive Biology of Amphibians. New York Plenum Press, New York, 233-262. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4757-6781-0_7 Wells KD (2007) The ecology and behaviour of amphibians. University of Chicago Press, Chicago, 1400 pp. Wiens JA (1989) The ecology of bird communities, Vol. 1. Foundations and patterns. Cambridge Univeristy Press, Cambridge, 539 pp. Wilczynski W, Chu J (2001) Acoustic communication, endocrine control, and the neurochemical system of the brain. In: Ryan MJ (Ed.) Anuran communication. Smithsonian Institute Press, Washington DC, 23-35. Wiley RH, Richards DG (1978) Physical constrains on acoustic com- munication in the atmosphere: Implications for the evolution of an- imal vocalization. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 3: 69-94. https://do1.org/10.1007/BF00300047 Zoosyst. Evol. 96 (2) 2020, 515-525 Williams SE, Hero JM (1998) Rainforest frogs of the Australian wet tropics: guild classification and the ecological similarity of declining species. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, Biological Sciences 265: 597-602. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb. 1998.0336 Wollenberg KC, Harvey J (2010) First assessment of the territorial vo- cal behaviour of Malagasy leaf litter frog (Gephyromantis thelenae). Herpetology Notes 3: 141-150. Wycherley J, Doran S, Beebee TJC (2002) Frog calls echo microsat- ellite phylogeography in the European pool frog (Rana lessonae). Journal of Zoology 258: 479-484. https://doi.org/10.1017/ S0952836902001632 Yager DD (1992) Underwater acoustic communication in the African pipid frog Xenopus borealis. Bioacoustics 4: 1-24. https://doi.org/1 0.1080/09524622.1992.9753201 Zheng Y, Dingqi RAO, Murphy RW, Zheng X (2011) Reproductive be- havior and underwater calls in the emei mustache toad, Leptobrachi- um boringii. Asian Herpetological Research 2: 199-215. https://doi. org/10.3724/SP.J.1245.2011.00199 Zweifel RG (1968) Effects of temperature, body size, and hybridization on mating calls of toads, Bufo a. americanus and Bufo woodhousii fowleri. Copeia 1968: 269-285. https://doi.org/10.2307/1441753 925 Supplementary material | Database for the definition of anuran call guilds Authors: Mike Emmrich, Miguel Vences, Raffael Ernst, Jorn Kohler, Michael F. Barej, Frank Glaw, Martin Jansen, Mark-Oliver Rodel Data type: DOCX file Explanation note: Table $1: List of 1253 anuran species with advertisement calls; References (literature and CDs) for anuran calls used in this study; Web sources for anuran calls used in this study. Copyright notice: This dataset is made available under the Open Database License (http://opendatacommons. org/licenses/odbl/1.0/). The Open Database License (ODbL) is a license agreement intended to allow us- ers to freely share, modify, and use this Dataset while maintaining this same freedom for others, provided that the original source and author(s) are credited. Link: https://doi.org/10.3897/zse.96.38770.suppl 1 zse.pensoft.net