A peer-reviewed open-access journal NeoBiota 87: 191—212 (2023) doi: 10.3897/neobiota.87. 108457 RESEARCH ARTICLE &) NeoBiota https:/ / neobi ota. pen soft. net Advancing research on alien species and biological invasions Predation risk by largemouth bass modulates feeding functional responses of native and non-native crayfish Larissa Faria'’, Jean R. S. Vitule', Julian D. Olden? | Laboratorio de Ecologia e Conservacaéo, Departamento de Engenharia Ambiental, Setor de Tecnologia, Uni- versidade Federal do Parand, Curitiba, 81531-980, Paranda, Brazil 2 School of Aquatic and Fishery Sciences, University of Washington, Seattle, 98195, Washington, USA Corresponding author: Larissa Faria (larissa.faria@fulbrightmail.org) Academic editor: Jaimie T.A. Dick | Received 22 June 2023 | Accepted 1 August 2023 | Published 4 October 2023 Citation: Faria L, Vitule JRS, Olden JD (2023) Predation risk by largemouth bass modulates feeding functional responses of native and non-native crayfish. NeoBiota 87: 191-212. https://doi.org/10.3897/neobiota.87.108457 Abstract Context-dependency is prevalent in nature, challenging our understanding and prediction of the potential ecological impacts of non-native species (NNS). The presence of a top predator, for example, can modify the foraging behaviour of an intermediate consumer, by means of non-consumptive effects. This raises the question of whether the fear of predation might modulate consumption rates of NNS, thus shaping the magnitude of ecological impacts. Here, we quantified the functional feeding responses of three non-native crayfish species — red swamp crayfish Procambarus clarkii, rusty crayfish Faxonius rusticus and virile crayfish Faxonius virilis — compared to the native analogue signal crayfish Pacifastacus leniusculus, considering the predation risk imposed by a top fish predator, the globally invasive largemouth bass Micropterus salmoides. We applied the comparative functional response (FR) approach using snails as prey and exposing crayfish to water containing predator and dietary chemical cues or not. All crayfish species presented a destabilising Type I FR, regardless of the presence of chemical cues. Predation risk resulted in significantly longer han- dling times or lower attack rates in non-native crayfish; however, no significant differences were observed in signal crayfish. We estimated per capita impacts for each species using the functional response ratio (FRR; attack rate divided by handling time). The FRR metric was lower for all crayfish species when ex- posed to predation risk. Rusty crayfish demonstrated the highest FRR in the absence of chemical cues, fol- lowed by signal crayfish, virile crayfish and red swamp crayfish. By contrast, the FRR of signal crayfish was nearly twice that of rusty crayfish and virile crayfish and ten times greater than red swamp crayfish when chemical cues were present. The latter result agrees with the well-recognised ecological impacts of signal crayfish throughout its globally-introduced range. This study demonstrates the importance of consider- ing the non-consumptive effects of predators when quantifying the ecological impacts of intermediate non-native consumers on prey. The direction and magnitude of the modulating effects of predators have clear implications for our understanding of NNS impacts and the prioritisation of management actions. Copyright Larissa Faria et al. This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY 4.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited. 192 Larissa Faria et al. / NeoBiota 87: 191-212 (2023) Keywords ecology of fear, decapods, higher-order predator, kairomones, trait-mediated indirect effects Introduction Non-native species (NNS) are a primary driver of environmental change, with negative impacts on individuals to entire ecosystems and severely disrupting important services provided by nature (Ricciardi et al. 2013). Economic burdens are also concerning, with estimated costs to prevent and control NNS impacts exceeding a hundred million dollars per year globally (Pysek et al. 2020; Diagne et al. 2021), possibly increasing in the future (Ahmed et al. 2022). Management and policy strategies rely on identify- ing the most impactful species to help target prevention efforts and allocate limited resources to control or eradicate burgeoning populations. Still, this task is challenging due to variations in NNS impacts, based on ecosystem, geographical location, time since establishment and individual values (Zavorka et al. 2018; Santos et al. 2019). Considering the increasing rate of invasions, comparative studies of the context-de- pendency of NNS impacts will help prioritise which species should be managed in the future (Lockwood et al. 2007; Dick et al. 2017a; Seebens et al. 2017). Quantifying per capita effects of NNS remains central to most frameworks evalu- ating their ecological impacts (Parker et al. 1999; Kumschick et al. 2015; Griffen et al. 2020). Given the challenges of estimating per capita effects, focus has shifted to the use of experiments to quantify resource consumption rates as a proxy (Dick et al. 2017b). Non-native consumers often consume resources more efhciently than their native counterparts (Funk and Vitousek 2007; Salo et al. 2007; Paolucci et al. 2013), making comparative rates of consumption between native and NNS a useful currency to anticipate negative impacts from species introductions (Dick et al. 2014). The fundamental ecological concept of functional responses (FR) — resource use as a function of availability — provides a measurable estimate of the per capita effect of a consumer on a given resource (Solomon 1949; Holling 1959; Dick et al. 2014). Type I FR describes a linear relationship between consumption and resource availabil- ity, typical of filter-feeding species (Jeschke et al. 2004). Type II FRs are destabilising due to high consumption rates at low resource densities, while Type III FRs promote stabilising effects due to low consumption rates when resources are scarce (Oaten and Murdoch 1975). The utility of FRs lies in comparing the maximum consumption rate between NNS and native trophic analogues in the same environmental context (Dick et al. 2014, 2017a), making this approach increasingly applied to quantify and predict ecological impacts of NNS (Faria et al. 2023). The comparative FR approach enables the evaluation of per capita effects in dif- ferent contexts, allowing more realistic and practical impact assessments (Dick et al. 2017a; Dickey et al. 2020). Despite this, investigations involving trophic levels be- yond the focal consumer-resource interaction remain limited (e.g. Barrios-O’Neill et al. (2014); Paterson et al. (2015)). Foraging behaviour and consumption rates of Predation risk modulates functional responses of non-native crayfish 193 consumers are sensitive to the presence of higher-order predators, which invoke trade- offs in resource acquisition versus mortality risk by predation (Lima and Dill 1990; Brown and Kotler 2004). Fear of predation can shape entire ecosystems through trait- mediated indirect effects (TMIEs) on prey traits, such as behaviour and physiology (Werner and Peacor 2003; Peckarsky et al. 2008; Laundré et al. 2010). In some cir- cumstances, the non-consumptive effects of predators are thought to be as strong as di- rect consumption for population dynamics, leading to greater system stability (Brown et al. 1999) or even causing trophic cascades (Schmitz et al. 2004; Preisser et al. 2005; Peckarsky et al. 2008). Trait-mediated indirect effects are particularly prominent in freshwater ecosys- tems, likely due to the effective transmission of visual and chemical cues indicating predator presence (Preisser et al. 2005). For example, the presence of largemouth bass (Micropterus salmoides) altered the foraging behaviour and habitat use of bluegill sunfish (Lepomis macrochirus) prey leading to cascading changes in zooplankton com- munities (Turner and Mittelbach 1990). In another example, rusty crayfish (Faxonius rusticus) displayed greater consumption of macrophytes when exposed to chemical cues from largemouth bass, suggesting a robust effect of predation risk on crayfish foraging behaviour (Wood et al. 2018). Despite the strong effects of non-consumptive effects in shaping communities, they are relatively underexplored compared to consumptive effects in the context of quan- tifying NNS impacts. Applying the comparative FR approach, we aim to test whether the non-consumptive effects of a top predator, the non-native largemouth bass, medi- ate the consumptive impacts of three non-native crayfish species (Procambarus clarkii, Faxonius virilis and E rusticus) and a native analogue (Pacifastacus leniusculus) preying on snails. We hypothesise that non-consumptive effects of a top predator will reduce consumption rates of all crayfish species, but to a lesser extent for non-native crayfish with a shorter evolutionary history with the predator. The differential response to pre- dation risk imposed by the largemouth bass may explain the expected higher per capita effects of non-native consumers compared to native analogue species. Methods Study system Our study system is a three-level food chain composed by a non-native top predator, the largemouth bass, an intermediate consumer represented by non-native or native crayfish (Table 1) and native freshwater snails (Gastropoda, Planorbidae) as the basal resource. Crayfish are known to be highly sensitive to different chemical cues such as predator odour, dietary and alarm cues (Beattie and Moore 2018; Wood et al. 2018; Wood and Moore 2020a, b) and these cues show utility in assessing TMIEs (Paterson et al. 2013). Thus, we used a combination of predator and dietary chemical cues to provide the biological context of predation risk in our comparative FR approach. 194 Larissa Faria et al. / NeoBiota 87: 191—212 (2023) Table |. Crayfish species examined in this study, including scientific and common names, history of in- troduction in the Pacific Northwest region, sampled populations (coordinates) and carapace length (CL) and mass, presented as the mean (SD), of the individuals used in the experiments. Crayfish Scientific Commonname_ Estimated time Sampled population CL Mass name of introduction (mm) (g) Pacifastacus Signal crayfish Native Skykomish River, WA 50.2 36.9 leniusculus (47.8482, -121.8403) (4.3) (10.3) Procambarus Red swamp 1970s Pine Lake, WA 5300 wy FS 9:7 clarkii crayfish (47.5907, -122.0389) (5.7) (13.0) Faxonius Rusty crayfish 2005 Magone Lake, OR 41.1 259 rusticus (44.5486, -118.9119) (3.1) (5.3) x Faxonius Virile crayfish 1980s Rattlesnake Lake, WA 46.0 32.6 ; virilis (47.4308, -121.7715) (3.5) (7.6) The geographic context of the study is the Pacific Northwest region of the US, where all species were sourced (Table 1). The apex predator largemouth bass has a native distribution that extends from north-eastern US to northern Mexico (Brown et al. 2009), with a long history of intentional introduction for recreational fishing in many regions of the US and the world, including the study region dating back to the beginning of the 20" century (Stein 1970). The non-native crayfish used in this study have a varied history of introductions in the region (Table 1) and are amongst the most widespread and harmful invasive crayfish species in the world (Twardochleb et al. 2013). Signal crayfish is the most widely distributed native crayfish species in the region (Larson and Olden 2011) and also highly invasive outside its native range (Usio et al. 2007; Twardochleb et al. 2013; VaefSen and Hollert 2015). All crayfish species used in this study are omnivorous and noctur- nal, presenting maximum feeding or growth rates between 20 and 30 °C (Crandall and Buhay 2008; Westhoff and Rosenberger 2016; Rodriguez Valido et al. 2021; Ruokonen and Karjalainen 2022). Predation risk modulates functional responses of non-native crayfish 195 Animal collection and maintenance Largemouth bass were collected using electrofishing from Lake Washington, WA (47.6469, -122.2991) in October 2022. A total of 33 fish were captured and trans- ported to the lab facility at the University of Washington, where they were maintained in a circular tank of approximately 800 | without shelter (hereafter stimulus tank), aer- ated and continuously filled with water from Lake Washington, in an open circulation system (mean total length = 194 mm, SD = 53). Fish were acclimatised to the stimulus tank for two weeks before the beginning of the trials. A total of 433 crayfish were sampled using baited traps deployed overnight from lakes in Washington and Oregon States in October 2022 (Table 1). Crayfish were kept in tanks of 256 | separated by species, with a maximum stock density of 30 individuals per tank. Stock tanks were continuously filled with water from Lake Washington, in an open circulation system and contained abundant structure for shelter. Crayfish were fed daily with commercial algae pellets until satiation and were acclimatised for at least two weeks before being used in the experiment. Snails used as prey were obtained from various commercial pet retailers (shell length mean = 10.3 mm, SD = 2.4). Snails were kept in a separate tank, in the same conditions as crayfish. Functional response experiments Native and NNS of crayfish were tested for differences in their predatory rate of snail prey supplied in seven different initial densities (2, 4, 8, 12, 16, 24 and 40 snails) under the presence or absence of waterborne predator and dietary chemical cues (here- after, predator treatment and control, respectively). Experiments were conducted in a fully-randomised design with respect to crayfish species and initial prey densities assigned to predator treatment and control. Experimental arenas were round opaque tanks (44.5 cm diameter, 42.5 cm height) filled with 10 | of water and no substrate or shelter were provided (Fig. 1A). At the predator treatment, water was supplied from the stimulus tank containing water from Lake Washington and bass (Fig. 1B), whereas un- der the control, just water from Lake Washington was supplied (as in the stock tanks) (Fig. 1C). Lake Washington water is piped directly from 10 m depth where largemouth bass and other fish species are at low abundance. Water was supplied continuously by dripping through small hoses (5 mm diameter) to ensure that chemical cues were pre- sent throughout the experiment (Fig. 1D). Water temperature in experimental arenas and stock tanks were similar, all demonstrating natural diel ranges of 12.5°-18.5 °C. Fish were starved for a week and then fed every other day a diet of crayfish before and during the experiments. Small individuals of all crayfish species were supplied simultaneously until satiation to enhance the response of crayfish to conspecific di- etary cues released by the fish (Beattie and Moore 2018; Wood et al. 2018). Crayfish were starved in a separate tank for 48 h before use in experiments to standardise hun- ger levels. After the starvation period, an individual crayfish was allocated to each 196 Larissa Faria et al. / NeoBiota 87: 191-212 (2023) Figure |. Experimental setup used in the functional response trials A experimental arena with a signal crayfish and snail prey during a trial B water from the tank containing largemouth bass (left) was pumped to a head tank (upper right) and C distributed to the experimental arenas in the predator treatment (upper-left row) or water was supplied directly from Lake Washington to the control arenas (lower-right row). Blue tanks in the background were stock and starvation tanks where crayfish were kept before being used in trials D the water from both treatments was supplied to each experimental arena via individually controlled hoses. experimental arena containing one of the seven initial densities of prey and allowed to forage for 24 h. The number of remaining prey was recorded at the end of the trials, along with the number of attacked, but uneaten prey. Crayfish sex, carapace length (CL) and mass were recorded, as well as the water temperature at the end of the trial. There were seven replicates for each combination of crayfish species, initial density of prey and treatment. At least five replicates of each combination were performed in the absence of crayfish to account for any background mortality of prey. Prey survi- vorship in these replicates was 99.9%, thus all prey deaths during experiments were attributed to crayfish predation. Crayfish were not reused in the experimental trials and replicates where crayfish moulted during the trial or one week after were repeated. Data analysis All statistical analyses were carried out in R version 4.1.2 (R Core Team 2021). Func- tional response analyses were conducted using the number of prey consumed as the response variable, under the frair package (Pritchard et al. 2017). For each crayfish species x treatment combination, FR type was determined by logistic regression of the proportion of prey consumed against initial prey density (Juliano 2001). If the proportion of prey consumed decreases with increasing prey density, it produces a significantly negative first-order term, indicating a Type II FR; if otherwise, a signifi- cantly positive first-order term is obtained then it indicates a Type III FR (Juliano 2001). When the results of the logistic regression were not conclusive, different FR Predation risk modulates functional responses of non-native crayfish 197 models were fitted directly and compared using Akaike’s Information Criterion (AIC) (Pritchard et al. 2017). Based on these analyses, all FRs were then modelled as Type H. Maximum Like- lihood model fitting was used to fit data to the Rogers’ random predator equation (Rogers 1972) that accounts for the depletion of prey along the experimental duration: N,=N (1 —exp(a(Nh- T))) where WV is the number of prey consumed, J, is the initial density of prey, a is the at- tack rate, / is the handling time and 7 is the time available for predation in days (i.e. experimental duration). As NV. is obtained experimentally, the estimated FR parameters are attack rate and handling time, representing a measure of successful attacks and the time needed for a predator to handle and ingest a prey item, respectively. The Lambert W function is implemented to solve the fact that V appears on both sides of the equa- tion (Bolker 2008). To compare FR parameters a and / between predator treatment and control, we used the indicator variables method (Juliano 2001; Pritchard et al. 2017), as: 0 =Ny—Noexpi[a + D,(/) [p+ DAW) |(N.)- TH-N, where j is an indicator variable that takes value 0 for control and 1 for predator treat- ment. The parameters Da and D/ estimate the differences between treatments in the value of the parameters a and /, respectively. If Da and Df are significantly different from zero, then the estimated FR parameters differ between treatment and control (Juliano 2001). The maximum feeding rate of each crayfish species x treatment com- bination was calculated as 1/(/T) indicating the maximum number of prey that one crayfish can consume in one day (T = 1 day). Additionally, the functional response ratio (FRR) was calculated, as a comparative metric of the ecological impact of NNS (Cuthbert et al. 2019), dividing the attack rate parameter by the handling time (a/A). High values of FRR are indicative of strong per capita impacts, while low values indi- cate less impactful predators (Cuthbert et al. 2019). Potential differences in the trial’s water temperature amongst species and initial densities of prey were evaluated through Kruskal-Wallis tests, as well as differences in crayfish weight and carapace length (CL) amongst species and initial densities of prey. Water temperature in trials did not vary amongst species (Kruskal—Wallis X?(3) = 7.366, p = 0.06) nor in association with the initial densities of prey tested (Kruskal-Wallis X7(6) = 11.339, p = 0.08). Crayfish mass and carapace length varied amongst species (Mass: Kruskal-Wallis X°(3) = 96.73, p < 0.001; CL: Kruskal—Wal- lis X°(3) = 224.73, p < 0.001). The effect of crayfish size and sex on the proportion of prey consumed was investigated with Spearman’s correlation and Mann-Whitney tests, respectively. Crayfish size (Mass: r = 0.01, p = 0.84; CL: r = -0.08, p = 0.12) and sex (Mann-Whitney U = 14828, p = 0.74) had no relation to the proportion of prey consumed. 198 Larissa Faria et al. / NeoBiota 87: 191-212 (2023) Results All crayfish species presented a destabilising Type II FR towards snail prey, regardless of the presence of chemical cues (Fig. 2). This was confirmed by the significantly negative first-order term of the logistic regression, except for red swamp crayfish under predator treatment and rusty crayfish under control, where the estimates were non-significant (Table 2). For these two specific cases, the FR type was determined by comparing the AIC of different model fittings. For red swamp crayfish, Type I had a lower AIC (AAIC = 0.8), while for rusty crayfish, Type II presented a better fit (AAIC = 1.1) compared to other models (generalised FR model, Type I and Type HI). Non-consumptive effects were observed for all non-native crayfish species, except for native signal crayfish (Fig. 2). Predation risk lowered the magnitude of the FR, which reflects reduced maximum consumption rates estimated in the predator treat- ment compared to the control (Table 2). These differences were driven by significantly longer handling time of snails for virile crayfish (Dh = -0.03 = 0.008, p < 0.001) and rusty crayfish (D/ = -0.02 + 0.008, p = 0.01) (Fig. 3A) and significantly lower attack rate displayed by red swamp crayfish (Da = 0.25 + 0.09, p = 0.006) (Fig. 3B) in the presence of chemical cues. No significant differences in handling time or attack rates were evidenced for signal crayfish between treatment and control. Native signal crayfish demonstrated a greater consumption rate when exposed to predation risk compared to non-native crayfish (Fig. 2). Per capita effects of signal crayfish on snails, according to the FRR, was nearly twice that of rusty crayfish and virile crayfish and ten times greater than red swamp crayfish (Fig. 4, Table 2). By con- trast, rusty crayfish demonstrated the highest FRR in the control, followed by signal crayfish, virile crayfish and red swamp crayfish (Fig. 4, Table 2). Discussion Predators can exert non-consumptive effects on prey that are comparable in magni- tude to consumptive effects (Werner and Peacor 2003; Preisser et al. 2005). Despite that, non-consumptive effects remain largely under-studied in evaluations of ecologi- cal impacts of NNS (Sih et al. 2010). Here, we quantified rates of snail predation by multiple non-native and a native crayfish species in the presence or absence of chemical and dietary cues from a higher-order predator. We found that predation risk reduced maximum consumption rates of snails due to longer handling times or lower attack rates, but did not alter the shape of the FR curve. Reduced foraging activity is a com- mon antipredator behavioural response of crayfish when exposed to predation risk (Gherardi et al. 2011b; Beattie and Moore 2018; Kenison et al. 2018). For example, red swamp crayfish significantly reduces the time spent feeding by adopting a lowered posture after being exposed to largemouth bass cues (Gherardi et al. 201 1b). Native signal crayfish was the only study species demonstrating little evidence for the effect of predation risk on the FR magnitude. This outcome is supported by a body Predation risk modulates functional responses of non-native crayfish 199 A) Signal crayfish (native) B) Red swamp crayfish =) oO t t —e— Predator --@-- Control 30 30 j=) ™N ra 2 - — > ~ ° oO bs ye — O = xe) cb) © Se eh Ce = 0 8 16 24 32 40 Cc C) Rusty crayfish D) Virile crayfish 8 io) =) \ ay + + \ oO ‘ — oO. 6 = 8 @ a “r = 0 = A. Zz oO a ; i=) N a NI = () 0 8 16 24 32 40 Initial density of prey (snails) Figure 2. Functional responses of native and non-native crayfish feeding on snails under predator treat- ment and control A native signal crayfish B non-native red swamp crayfish C non-native rusty crayfish and D non-native virile crayfish. Lines represent model fit (solid line: predator treatment, dashed line: control). Points represent mean consumption and error bars represent + SE per density (filled circles: predator treatment, open circles: control; n = 7 per initial density x treatment combination). of literature suggesting that the response of signal crayfish to predation risk is highly var- iable and context-dependent (Stebbing et al. 2010; Gherardi et al. 2011b; Beattie and Moore 2018; Wood and Moore 2020b). For instance, Stebbing et al. (2010) observed altered behaviour as raised posture in signal crayfish exposed to the chemical cues of European eels (Anguilla anguilla), but not to European perch (Perca fluviatilis), whereas Hirvonen et al. (2007) reported crayfish reduced shelter use when exposed to eel odour. 200 Larissa Faria et al. / NeoBiota 87: 191-212 (2023) Table 2. Functional response estimates of native and non-native crayfish species under predator treat- ment and control. The 1* order term of the logistic regression (see Methods), the functional response (FR) type, estimated parameters attack rate (a) and handling time (/), the maximum feeding rate (1//T) and the functional response ratio (FRR). * = significant results. Treatment/ Species 1 order term FR at SE (p-value) 4+ SE (p-value) 1/AT FRR (p-value) type Predator Signal crayfish (native) -0.0617 II 2.26 + 0.23 0.04 + 0.004 28.02 63.5 (> 0.001)* (> 0.001)* (> 0.001)* Red swamp crayfish -0.0078 i 0.25 + 0.05 0.04 + 0.037 24.81 6.1 (0.29) (> 0.001)* (0.271) Rusty crayfish -0.0283 II 1.16 +0.14 0.03 + 0.006 31,37 36.3 (> 0.001)* (> 0.001)* (> 0.001)* Virile crayfish -0.0411 II 1.64 + 0.23 0.05 + 0.006 19.81 32.5 (> 0.001)* (> 0.001)* (> 0.001)* Control Signal crayfish (native) -0.0358 II 2.08 + 0.24 0.03 = 0.004 37.53 78.2 (> 0.001)* (> 0.001)* (> 0.001)* Red swamp crayfish -0.0142 a 0.50 + 0.08 0.04 + 0.015 2 14.3 (0.022) (> 0.001)* (0.021)* Rusty crayfish -0.0101 Ul 1.14 + 0.13 0.01 + 0.006 94.99 108.0 (0.086) (> 0.001)* (0.061) Virile crayfish -0.0252 II 1.43 + 0.15 0.02 + 0.005 47.95 68.8 (> 0.001)* (> 0.001)* (> 0.001)* "Despite being categorised as Type I using AIC, we fitted data to the Type II model to allow comparison of parameters between treatment and control. ‘These studies were performed in the invaded range of signal crayfish; thus, unexpected behavioural responses were attributed to naive juvenile individuals with a lack of evo- lutionary history with these predators (Hirvonen et al. 2007; Stebbing et al. 2010). However, this may not be the case in our study where signal crayfish is native and has experience with largemouth bass in the region for over a hundred years. ‘This suggests that signal crayfish may better assess the risk posed by a familiar predator using both chemical and visual cues (Blake and Hart 1993), whereas all three non-native crayfish species responded in a more conservative manner to the presence of chemical cues alone in a novel environment (Gherardi et al. 2002; Hazlett et al. 2003). Another possible mechanism is related to the larger body sizes of signal crayfish compared to other species (both in the wild and individuals used in this experiment: Table 1). There is evidence that crayfish can assess predator size through chemical cues (Wood and Moore 2020a; Wagner and Moore 2022); thus, the size of the largemouth bass in the experiment may have been too small relative to signal crayfish to elicit an antipredator behaviour resulting in reduced foraging rates. All crayfish species presented a Type II FR, which is deemed to destabilise re- source populations. This result aligns with the known impacts of these species on bio- mass and abundance of benthic invertebrates, particularly snails (Twardochleb et al. 2013). Moreover, when applying an FR-based metric to evaluate impacts, our native Predation risk modulates functional responses of non-native crayfish 201 @® Predator © Control 0.06 0.04 0.02 Handling time (day/prey) 0.00 Kk Attack rate (arena/day) 0.5 p é Signal Red Rusty Virile crayfish swamp. crayfish crayfish (native) crayfish Figure 3. Estimated functional response parameters of native and non-native crayfish species under predator treatment and control A handling time parameter / and B attack rate parameter a. Points rep- resent the mean estimate of the model (filled circles: predator treatment, open circles: control) and error bars represent + SE. *p < 0.1 and **p < 0.05. comparator species generally showed a higher FRR than non-natives, which con- tradicts the pattern of invaders being more impactful than their native counterparts (Cuthbert et al. 2019). Nevertheless, signal crayfish is itself highly invasive in Europe, Japan and the south-western United States, usually reaching higher abundances than those observed in its native range (Larson and Olden 2013) and causing significant impact through their omnivorous feeding habits (Usio et al. 2009; Twardochleb et al. 2013; VaefSen and Hollert 2015). Our results also align with a previous study that 202 Larissa Faria et al. / NeoBiota 87: 191-212 (2023) 160 B® Signal crayfish (native) NM) Red swamp crayfish || Rusty crayfish BW Virile crayfish 120 80 Functional response ratio (FRR) Predator Control Figure 4. The functional response ratio (FRR) of native and non-native crayfish species under predator treatment and control. The calculated FRR (a/h) is represented as bars (solid bars: predator treatment, shadowed bars: control) and error bars represent propagated standard errors of original estimates of pa- rameters attack rate a and handling time /. experimentally compared the predation rate of signal crayfish and non-native crayfish towards Chinese mystery snail (Bellamya chinensis), where native signal crayfish con- sumed significantly more snails of all size classes than did non-native crayfish (Olden et al. 2009). Indeed, previous studies that used the comparative FR approach to assess the impacts of signal crayfish where it is non-native found that the species generally present higher FR magnitude when compared to European native analogues, such as white-clawed crayfish (Austropotamobius pallipes) and noble crayfish (Astacus astacus), although impact varied with prey type (Haddaway et al. 2012; Rosewarne et al. 2016; Taylor and Dunn 2018; Chucholl and Chucholl 2021). Differences in experimental systems, such as diverse prey types and arena sizes, preclude us from comparing the na- tive signal crayfish findings here to those of invasive populations of the species. Future comparative studies of the FRs of native and non-native populations are recommended. We found significant differences amongst NNS predatory impacts towards prey. Rusty crayfish and virile crayfish showed consumption rates similar to those of native signal crayfish, whereas red swamp crayfish demonstrated the lowest feeding rate, despite the latter species being considered one of the most impactful invasive crayfish in the world (Lodge et al. 2012; Twardochleb et al. 2013). Even though all crayfish are con- sidered omnivorous or polytrophic, there are marked differences in their predominant trophic ecology (Reynolds et al. 2013). For instance, red swamp crayfish has a lower trophic position than signal crayfish, which is consistent with the perceived impact on macrophyte communities of the former species (Matsuzaki et al. 2009; Larson et al. 2017). Similarly, Madzivanzira et al. (2021) also reported a lower FR magnitude of red swamp crayfish preying on catfish fry compared to a native analogue crab. Despite this, it Predation risk modulates functional responses of non-native crayfish 203 has been demonstrated that invasive populations of red swamp crayfish in Europe present ontogenetic niche shifts and have opportunistic feeding habits, adjusting its diet to differ- ent biotic and abiotic contexts, which further explains its success as an invader (Correia 2002, 2003; Carreira et al. 2017; Jackson et al. 2017). Additionally, red swamp crayfish has weaker chelae closing force compared to other decapods, which helps explain its preference for feeding on softer resources (South et al. 2020). Our findings support previ- ously observed impacts of rusty crayfish and virile crayfish on snail communities where they are invasive (Dorn and Wojdak 2004; McCarthy et al. 2006; Kreps et al. 2012). The greater consumption rate of invasive crayfish is likely associated with selected traits, such as boldness and foraging voracity in NNS populations, which are known to differ from their native range (Pintor and Sih 2009; Reisinger et al. 2017; Glon et al. 2018). Previous studies that investigated TMIEs using the FR approach reported mixed outcomes. Considering simple habitats, the presence of predator cues reduced con- sumption rates of the amphipod Echinogammarus marinus, an intermediate predator, towards isopod prey (Alexander et al. 2013). By contrast, fish cues did not influence the FR’s magnitude of two amphipod species (the native Gammarus duebeni and the inva- sive Gammarus pulex) towards three different invertebrate preys (Paterson et al. 2015). Our study reinforces the need for considering the wider biological context of ecological interactions when quantifying the impacts of NNS. Moving forward, we suggest three primary ways that future studies could further explore context-dependencies. First, the effect of abiotic contexts, such as habitat complexity and presence of shelter, continues to be a research need. The Type II FR curves reported here align with general expectations from the broader literature (Jeschke et al. 2004); however, the lack of habitat complexity in experimental arenas may prevent the observation of more stabilising Type III responses (Alexander et al. 2012; Griffen 2021). Likewise, gravel substrate has been reported to reduce crayfish consumption of pelagic, but not benthic prey (Vollmer and Gall 2014; South et al. 2019). Additionally, shelter use is a common behavioural response of crayfish to predator cues (Blake and Hart 1993; Garvey et al. 1994) and could have further magnified the observed differences between the predator treatment and control reported in our study. Second, it would be valuable to evaluate additional biotic contexts, such as alter- native resource availability, the presence of intra- and inter-specific competitors and effects of visual predator cues. For instance, prey preference for different resources, such as macrophyte or detritus, could have a significant effect on FRs for omnivorous crayfish (Cuthbert et al. 2018; Médoc et al. 2018), ultimately defining their ecologi- cal impacts when invasive (South et al. 2019; Chucholl and Chucholl 2021). Better incorporating the effects of competitive interactions in FR experiments are also fun- damental to more realistic scaling of NNS per capita effects in the wild (Dickey et al. 2020; Latombe et al. 2022). As crayfish can respond to predation risk using a variety of different cues, the relative importance of visual and alarm cues can also be investigated using FRs (Blake and Hart 1993; Ramberg-Pihl and Yurewicz 2020). Third, future research investigations discussed above would benefit from the linking of mesocosms experiments with in-situ field studies to ensure robust scaling of our understanding (Iacarella et al. 2018). 204 Larissa Faria et al. / NeoBiota 87: 191-212 (2023) Conclusions Ecological impacts of NNS are notoriously challenging to anticipate given a myriad of biotic and abiotic context-dependencies that can affect the organismal performance in nature. The comparative FR approach has been used to incorporate these context- dependencies to predict the impact of NNS, through relative comparisons of per capita effects (Dick et al. 2014; Cuthbert et al. 2019; Faria et al. 2023). Here we showed that the presence of a higher-order predator can alter important parameters of FR, with direct effects on maximum consumption rates and predicted impact of intermediate non-native consumers. These findings suggest that the broader biological context in which consumer activities take place should not be overlooked if we aim to understand the ecological impacts of NNS. Likewise, biogeographic origin alone is not the sole indicator of impact, as we found that native signal crayfish demonstrated the highest estimated impact on prey in the presence of predation risk by a fish predator. The ecology of fear predicts that the cost of anti-predator behaviour is associated with reduced offspring, thus modulating consumer abundance (Zanette and Clinchy 2019). Given the immense challenges in eradicating and controlling invasive crayfish populations (Gherardi et al. 2011a; Manfrin et al. 2019), this raises the interesting question of whether chemical cues could be used as an additional management tool to reduce their short-term ecological impacts, while other control strategies are being implemented. We encourage more research on which and how chemical components of predator and dietary cues trigger behavioural responses in crayfish, as these are not entirely elucidated (Mitchell et al. 2017), but have potential management applications. Acknowledgements Assistance in the collection of fish provided by the Washington Department of Fish and Wildlife and the collection of crayfish by the Oregon Department of Fish and Wildlife, the Washington Crawfish Company and Dwayne Lamb. We thank Alexan- dre Bee Amaral, Jon Wittouck and lab-mates in the Freshwater Ecology & Conserva- tion Lab for their help in assembling the mesocosms. We are grateful to Fulbright Bra- zil, the Fulbright Scholarship Board and the Bureau of the Educational and Cultural Affairs of the United States Department of State for the scholarship granted to LF as part of the Doctoral Dissertation Research Award programme. JRSV was supported by Conselho Nacional de Desenvolvimento Cientifico e Tecnolégico (CNPq) — Process Numbers 02367/2018-7 and 303776/2015-3. JDO was supported by the Richard C. and Lois M. Worthington Endowed Professor in Fisheries Management from the School of Aquatic and Fishery Sciences, University of Washington. ‘This study was also supported by Coordenacao de Aperfeigoamento de Pessoal do Nivel Superior (CAPES) — Finance Code 001. All experiments were approved by the University of Washington Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee (permit 4172-12). Predation risk modulates functional responses of non-native crayfish 205 References Ahmed DA, Hudgins EJ, Cuthbert RN, Haubrock PJ, Renault D, Bonnaud E, Diagne C, Courchamp F (2022) Modelling the damage costs of invasive alien species. Biological Inva- sions 24(7): 1949-1972. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10530-02 1-02586-5 Alexander ME, Dick JTA, O’Connor NE, Haddaway NR, Farnsworth KD (2012) Functional responses of the intertidal amphipod Echinogammarus marinus: Effects of prey supply, model selection and habitat complexity. Marine Ecology Progress Series 468: 191-202. https://doi.org/10.3354/meps09978 Alexander ME, Dick JTA, O’Connor NE (2013) Trait-mediated indirect interactions in a ma- rine intertidal system as quantified by functional responses. Oikos 122(11): 1521-1531. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1600-0706.2013.00472.x Barrios-O’Neill D, Dick JTA, Emmerson MC, Ricciardi A, Maclsaac HJ, Alexander ME, Bovy HC (2014) Fortune favours the bold: A higher predator reduces the impact of a native but not an invasive intermediate predator. Journal of Animal Ecology 83(3): 693-701. https://doi.org/10.1111/1365-2656.12155 Beattie MC, Moore PA (2018) Predator recognition of chemical cues in crayfish: Diet and experience influence the ability to detect predation threats. Behaviour 155(6): 505-530. https://doi.org/10.1163/1568539X-00003501 Blake MA, Hart PJB (1993) The behavioural responses of juvenile signal crayfish Pacifastacus leniusculus to stimuli from perch and eels. Freshwater Biology 29(1): 89-97. https://doi. org/10.1111/j.1365-2427.1993.tb00747.x Bolker B (2008) Ecological Models and Data in R. Princeton University Press, Princeton, 408 pp. https://doi.org/10.1086/644667 Brown JS, Kotler BP (2004) Hazardous duty pay and the foraging cost of predation. Ecology Letters 7(10): 999-1014. https://doi-org/10.1111/j.1461-0248.2004.00661.x Brown JS, Laundré JW, Gurung M (1999) ‘The ecology of fear: Optimal foraging, game theory, and trophic interactions. Journal of Mammalogy 80(2): 385-399. https://doi. org/10.2307/1383287 Brown TG, Runciman B, Pollard S, Grant ADA (2009) Biological synopsis of largemouth bass (Micropterus salmoides). Canadian Manuscript Report of Fisheries and Aquatic Sciences 2884: 1-27. Carreira BM, Segurado P, Laurila A, Rebelo R (2017) Can heat waves change the trophic role of the world’s most invasive crayfish? Diet shifts in Procambarus clarkii. PLoS ONE 12(9): e0183108. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0183108 Chucholl K Chucholl C (2021) Differences in the functional responses of four invasive and one native crayfish species suggest invader-specific ecological impacts. Freshwater Biology 66(11): 2051-2063. https://doi.org/10.1111/fwb.13813 Correia AM (2002) Niche breadth and trophic diversity: Feeding behaviour of the red swamp crayfish (Procambarus clarkii) towards environmental availability of aquatic macroinverte- brates in a rice field (Portugal). Acta Oecologica 23(6): 421-429. https://doi.org/10.1016/ S$1146-609X(02)01 166-9 206 Larissa Faria et al. / NeoBiota 87: 191-212 (2023) Correia AM (2003) Food choice by the introduced crayfish Procambarus clarkii. Annales Zoo- logici Fennici 40: 517-528. Crandall KA, Buhay JE (2008) Global diversity of crayfish (Astacidae, Cambaridae, and Parastacidae — Decapoda) in freshwater. Hydrobiologia 595(1): 295-301. https://doi. org/10.1007/s10750-007-9 120-3 Cuthbert RN, Dickey JWE, McMorrow C, Laverty C, Dick JTA (2018) Resistance is futile: Lack of predator switching and a preference for native prey predict the success of an in- vasive prey species. Royal Society Open Science 5(8): e180339. https://doi.org/10.1098/ rsos. 180339 Cuthbert RN, Dickey JWE, Coughlan NE, Joyce PWS, Dick JTA (2019) The Functional Re- sponse Ratio (FRR): Advancing comparative metrics for predicting the ecological impacts of invasive alien species. Biological Invasions 21(8): 2543-2547. https://doi.org/10.1007/ s10530-019-02002-z Diagne C, Leroy B, Vaissi¢re AC, Gozlan RE, Roiz D, Jari¢ I, Salles JM, Bradshaw CJA, Cour- champ F (2021) High and rising economic costs of biological invasions worldwide. Nature 592(7855): 571-576. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41586-021-03405-6 Dick JTA, Alexander ME, Jeschke J, Ricciardi A, Maclsaac HJ, Robinson TB, Kumschick S, Weyl OLE Dunn AM, Hatcher MJ, Paterson RA, Farnsworth KD, Richardson DM (2014) Advancing impact prediction and hypothesis testing in invasion ecology using a com- parative functional response approach. Biological Invasions 16(4): 735-753. https://doi. org/10.1007/s10530-013-0550-8 Dick JTA, Alexander ME, Ricciardi A, Laverty C, Downey PO, Xu M, Jeschke J, Saul W-C, Hill MP, Wasserman R, Barrios-O’Neill D, Weyl OLE Shaw RH (2017a) Functional re- sponses can unify invasion ecology. Biological Invasions 19(5): 1667-1672. https://doi. org/10.1007/s10530-016-1355-3 Dick JTA, Laverty C, Lennon JJ, Barrios-O’ Neill D, Mensink PJ, Britton JR, Médoc V, Boets P, Alexander ME, Taylor NG, Dunn AM, Hatcher MJ, Rosewarne PJ, Crookes S, Maclsaac HJ, Xu M, Ricciardi A, Wasserman RJ, Ellender BR, Weyl OLE Lucy FE, Banks PB, Dodd JA, MacNeil C, Penk M, Aldridge DC, Caffrey JM (2017b) Invader Relative Im- pact Potential: A new metric to understand and predict the ecological impacts of existing, emerging and future invasive alien species. Journal of Applied Ecology 54(4): 1259-1267. https://doi.org/10.1111/1365-2664.12849 Dickey JWE, Cuthbert RN, South J, Britton JR, Caffrey J, Chang X, Crane K, Coughlan NE, Fadaei E, Farnsworth KD, Ismar-Rebitz SMH, Joyce PWS, Julius M, Laverty C, Lucy FE, Maclsaac HJ, McCard M, McGlade CLO, Reid N, Ricciardi A, Wasserman RJ, Weyl OLE Dick JTA (2020) On the RIP: Using Relative Impact Potential to assess the ecological impacts of invasive alien species. NeoBiota 55: 27—60. https://doi.org/10.3897/ neobiota.55.49547 Dorn NJ, Wojdak JM (2004) The role of omnivorous crayfish in littoral communities. Oecolo- gia 140(1): 150-159. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00442-004-1548-9 Faria L, Cuthbert RN, Dickey JWE, Jeschke JM, Ricciardi A, Dick JTA, Vitule JRS (2023) The rise of the Functional Response in invasion science: A systematic review. NeoBiota 85: 43-79. https://doi.org/10.3897/neobiota.85.98902 Predation risk modulates functional responses of non-native crayfish 207 Funk JL, Vitousek PM (2007) Resource-use efficiency and plant invasion in low-resource sys- tems. Nature 446(7139): 1079-1081. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature05719 Garvey JE, Stein RA, Thomas HM (1994) Assessing how fish predation and interspecific prey competition influence a crayfish assemblage. Ecology 75(2): 532-547. https://doi. org/10.2307/1939556 Gherardi F, Acquistapace P, Hazlett BA, Whisson G (2002) Behavioural responses to alarm odours in indigenous and non-indigenous crayfish species: A case study from Western Aus- tralia. Marine and Freshwater Research 53(1): 93-98. https://doi.org/10.1071/MF00131 Gherardi FE, Aquiloni L, Diéguez-Uribeondo J, Tricarico E (201 1a) Managing invasive crayfish: Is there a hope? Aquatic Sciences 73(2): 185—200. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00027-011-0181-z Gherardi EK Mavuti KM, Pacini IC, Tricarico E, Harper DM (2011b) The smell of danger: Chemical recognition of fish predators by the invasive crayfish Procambarus clarkii. Fresh- water Biology 56(8): 1567-1578. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2427.2011.02595.x Glon MG, Reisinger LS, Pintor LM (2018) Biogeographic differences between native and non-native populations of crayfish alter species coexistence and trophic interactions in mesocosms. Biological Invasions 20(12): 3475-3490. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10530- 018-1788-y Griffen BD (2021) Considerations when applying the consumer functional response measured under artificial conditions. Frontiers in Ecology and Evolution 9: e713147. https://doi. org/10.3389/fevo.2021.713147 Griffen BD, van den Akker D, DiNuzzo ER, Anderson L, Vernier A (2020) Comparing meth- ods for predicting the impacts of invasive species. Biological Invasions 23(2): 491-505. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10530-020-02377-4 Haddaway NR, Wilcox RH, Heptonstall REA, Griffiths HM, Mortimer RJG, Christmas M, Dunn AM (2012) Predatory functional response and prey choice identify predation dif- ferences between native/invasive and parasitised/unparasitised crayfish. PLoS ONE 7(2): e32229. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0032229 Hazlett BA, Burba A, Gherardi F, Acquistapace P (2003) Invasive species of crayfish use a broad- er range of predation-risk cues than native species. Biological Invasions 5(3): 223-228. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:10261 14623612 Hirvonen H, Holopainen S, Lempiainen N, Selin M, Tulonen J (2007) Sniffing the trade- off: Effects of eel odours on nocturnal foraging activity of native and introduced crayfish juveniles. Marine and Freshwater Behaviour and Physiology 40(3): 213-218. https://doi. org/10.1080/10236240701556919 Holling CS (1959) Some characteristics of simple types of predation and parasitism. Canadian Entomologist 91(7): 385-398. https://doi.org/10.4039/Ent91385-7 Tacarella JC, Hudgins EJ, Dick JTA, Ricciardi A (2018) Predatory behaviour of an invasive am- phipod in response to varying conspecific densities under higher-order predation risk. Ca- nadian Journal of Fisheries and Aquatic Sciences 75(1): 131-140. https://doi.org/10.1139/ cjfas-2016-0417 Jackson MC, Evangelista C, Zhao T, Lecerf A, Britton JR, Cucherousset J (2017) Between- lake variation in the trophic ecology of an invasive crayfish. Freshwater Biology 62(9): 1501-1510. https://doi.org/10.1111/fwb.12957 208 Larissa Faria et al. / NeoBiota 87: 191-212 (2023) Jeschke J, Kopp M, Tollrian R (2004) Consumer-food systems: Why type I functional respons- es are exclusive to filter feeders. Biological Reviews of the Cambridge Philosophical Society 79(2): 337-349. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1464793 103006286 Juliano SA (2001) Nonlinear curve fitting: Predation and functional response curves. In: Scheiner SM, Gurevitch J (Eds) Design and Analysis of Ecological Experiments. Oxford University Press, New York, 178-196. Kenison EK, Weldy PY, Williams RN (2018) There must be something in the water: Assess- ing the behavioral responses of rusty crayfish (Orconectes rusticus) to fish and amphibian predator kairomones. Journal of Ethology 36(1): 77-84. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10164- 017-0529-5 Kreps TA, Baldridge AK, Lodge DM (2012) The impact of an invasive predator (Orconectes rus- ticus) on freshwater snail communities: Insights on habitat-specific effects from a multilake long-term study. Canadian Journal of Fisheries and Aquatic Sciences 69(7): 1164-1173. https://doi.org/10.1139/f2012-052 Kumschick S, Gaertner M, Vila M, Essl E, Jeschke J, PySek P, Ricciardi A, Bacher S, Blackburn TM, Dick JTA, Evans T, Hulme PE, Kiithn I, Mrugala A, Pergl J, Rabitsch W, Richardson DM, Sendek A, Winter M (2015) Ecological impacts of alien species: Quantification, scope, cave- ats, and recommendations. Bioscience 65(1): 55-63. https://doi.org/10.1093/biosci/biu193 Larson ER, Olden JD (2011) The state of crayfish in the Pacific Northwest. Fisheries (Bethesda, Md.) 36(2): 60-73. https://doi.org/10.1577/03632415.2011.10389069 Larson ER, Olden JD (2013) Crayfish occupancy and abundance in lakes of the Pacific North- west, USA. Freshwater Science 32(1): 94-107. https://doi.org/10.1899/12-051.1 Larson ER, Twardochleb LA, Olden JD (2017) Comparison of trophic function between the globally invasive crayfishes Pacifastacus leniusculus and Procambarus clarkii. Limnology 18(3): 275-286. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10201-016-0505-8 Latombe G, Catford JA, Essl F, Lenzner B, Richardson DM, Wilson JRU, McGeoch MA (2022) GIRAE: A generalised approach for linking the total impact of invasion to spe- cies’ range, abundance and per-unit effects. Biological Invasions 24(10): 3147-3167. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10530-022-02836-0 Laundré JW, Hernandez L, Ripple WJ (2010) The landscape of fear: Ecologi- cal implications of being afraid. The Open Ecology Journal 3(3): 1-7. https://doi. org/10.2174/1874213001003030001 Lima SL, Dill LM (1990) Behavioral decisions made under the risk of predation: A review and prospectus. Canadian Journal of Zoology 68(4): 619-640. https://doi.org/10.1139/ z90-092 Lockwood JL, Hoopes M, Marchetti M (2007) Invasion Ecology (1* edn.). Blackwell Publish- ing, Oxford, 312 pp. Lodge DM, Deines A, Gherardi F, Yeo DCJ, Arcella T, Baldridge AK, Barnes MA, Chadderton WL, Feder JL, Gantz CA, Howard GW, Jerde CL, Peters BW, Peters JA, Sargent LW, Turner CR, Wittmann ME, Zeng Y (2012) Global introductions of crayfishes: Evaluating the im- pact of species invasions on ecosystem services. Annual Review of Ecology, Evolution, and Systematics 43(1): 449-472. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-ecolsys-111511-103919 Madzivanzira TC, South J, Weyl OLF (2021) Invasive crayfish outperform Potamonautid crabs at higher temperatures. Freshwater Biology 66(5): 978-991. https://doi.org/10.1111/fwb.13691 Predation risk modulates functional responses of non-native crayfish 209 Manfrin C, Souty-Grosset C, Anastacio PM, Reynolds J, Giulianini PG (2019) Detection and control of invasive freshwater crayfish: From traditional to innovative methods. Diversity (Basel) 11(1): 1-5. https://doi.org/10.3390/d11010005 Matsuzaki SIS, Usio N, Takamura N, Washitani I (2009) Contrasting impacts of invasive en- gineers on freshwater ecosystems: An experiment and meta-analysis. Oecologia 158(4): 673-686. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00442-008-1180-1 McCarthy JM, Hein CL, Olden JD, Jake Vander Zanden M (2006) Coupling long-term studies with meta-analysis to investigate impacts of non-native crayfish on zoobenthic communities. Freshwater Biology 51(2): 224-235. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365- 2427.2005.01485.x Médoc V, Thuillier L, Spataro T (2018) Opportunistic omnivory impairs our ability to predict invasive species impacts from functional response comparisons. Biological Invasions 20(5): 1307-1319. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10530-017-1628-5 Mitchell MD, Bairos-Novak KR, Ferrari MCO (2017) Mechanisms underlying the control of responses to predator odours in aquatic prey. The Journal of Experimental Biology 220(11): 1937-1946. https://doi.org/10.1242/jeb.135137 Oaten A, Murdoch WW (1975) Functional response and stability in predator-prey systems. American Naturalist 109(967): 289-298. https://doi.org/10.1086/282998 Olden JD, Larson ER, Mims MC (2009) Home-field advantage: Native signal crayfish (Paci- fastacus leniusculus) out consume newly introduced crayfishes for invasive Chinese mystery snail (Bellamya chinensis). Aquatic Ecology 43(4): 1073-1084. https://doi.org/10.1007/ s10452-009-9244-9 Paolucci EM, Maclsaac HJ, Ricciardi A (2013) Origin matters: Alien consumers inflict greater damage on prey populations than do native consumers. Diversity & Distributions 19(8): 988-995. https://doi.org/10.1111/ddi.12073 Parker IM, Simberloff D, Lonsdale W, Goodell K, Wonham M, Kareiva P, Williamson M, Von Holle B, Moyle P, Byers J, Goldwasser L (1999) Impact: Toward a framework for under- standing the ecological effects of invaders. Biological Invasions 1(1): 3-19. https://doi. org/10.1023/A:1010034312781 Paterson RA, Pritchard DW, Dick JTA, Alexander ME, Hatcher MJ, Dunn AM (2013) Preda- tor cue studies reveal strong trait-mediated effects in communities despite variation in experimental designs. Animal Behaviour 86(6): 1301-1313. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. anbehav.2013.09.036 Paterson RA, Dick JTA, Pritchard DW, Ennis M, Hatcher MJ, Dunn AM (2015) Predicting in- vasive species impacts: A community module functional response approach reveals context dependencies. Journal of Animal Ecology 84(2): 453-463. https://doi.org/10.1111/1365- 26560012292) Peckarsky BL, Abrams PA, Bolnick DI, Dill LM, Grabowski JH, Luttbeg B, Orrock JL, Peacor SD, Preisser EL, Schmitz OJ, Trussell GC (2008) Revisiting the classics: Considering non- consumptive effects in textbook examples of predator—prey interactions. Ecology 89(9): 2416-2425. https://doi.org/10.1890/07-1131.1 Pintor LM, Sih A (2009) Differences in growth and foraging behavior of native and introduced populations of an invasive crayfish. Biological Invasions 11(8): 1895-1902. https://doi. org/10.1007/s10530-008-9367-2 210 Larissa Faria et al. / NeoBiota 87: 191-212 (2023) Preisser EL, Bolnick DI, Benard MF (2005) Scared to death? The effects of intimidation and consumption in predator—prey interactions. Ecology 86(2): 501-509. https://doi. org/10.1890/04-0719 Pritchard DW, Paterson RA, Bovy HC, Barrios-O’Neill D (2017) frair: An R package for fitting and comparing consumer functional responses. Methods in Ecology and Evolution 8(11): 1528-1534. https://doi.org/10.1111/2041-210X.12784 PySek P, Hulme PE, Simberloff D, Bacher S, Blackburn TM, Carlton JT, Dawson W, Essl F, Foxcroft LC, Genovesi P, Jeschke JM, Kiihn I, Liebhold AM, Mandrak NE, Meyerson LA, Pauchard A, Pergl J, Roy HE, Seebens H, van Kleunen M, Vila M, Wingfield MJ, Rich- ardson DM (2020) Scientists’ warning on invasive alien species. Biological Reviews of the Cambridge Philosophical Society 95(6): 1511-1534. https://doi.org/10.1111/brv.12627 R Core Team (2021) R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna. https://www.R-project.org/ Ramberg-Pihl NC, Yurewicz KL (2020) Behavioral responses of northern crayfish (Faxonius virilis) to conspecific alarm cues and predator cues from smallmouth bass (Micropterus dolomieu). Marine and Freshwater Behaviour and Physiology 53(1): 1-16. https://doi.org /10.1080/10236244.2020.1717338 Reisinger LS, Elgin AK, Towle KM, Chan DJ, Lodge DM (2017) The influence of evolution and plasticity on the behavior of an invasive crayfish. Biological Invasions 19(3): 815-830. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10530-016-1346-4 Reynolds J, Souty-Grosset C, Richardson A (2013) Ecological roles of crayfish in freshwa- ter and terrestrial habitats. Freshwater Crayfish 19: 197-218. https://doi.org/10.5869/ fc.2013.v19-2.197 Ricciardi A, Hoopes ME, Marchetti MP, Lockwood JL (2013) Progress toward understand- ing the ecological impacts of nonnative species. Ecological Monographs 83(3): 263-282. https://doi.org/10.1890/13-0183.1 Rodriguez Valido CA, Johnson ME, Dugdale SJ, Cutts V, Fell HG, Higgins EA, Tarr S, Temp- ley CM, Algar AC (2021) Thermal sensitivity of feeding and burrowing activity of an inva- sive crayfish in UK waters. Ecohydrology 14(1): e2258. https://doi.org/10.1002/eco.2258 Rogers D (1972) Random search and insect population models. Journal of Animal Ecology 41(2): 369-383. https://doi.org/10.2307/3474 Rosewarne PJ, Mortimer RJG, Newton RJ, Grocock C, Wing CD, Dunn AM (2016) Feeding behaviour, predatory functional responses and trophic interactions of the invasive Chinese mitten crab (Eriocheir sinensis) and signal crayfish (Pacifastacus leniusculus). Freshwater Biology 61(4): 426-443. https://doi.org/10.1111/fwb.12717 Ruokonen TJ, Karjalainen J (2022) Divergent temperature-specific metabolic and feeding rates of native and invasive crayfish. Biological Invasions 24(3): 787-799. https://doi. org/10.1007/s10530-021-02687-1 Salo P, Korpimaki E, Banks PB, Nordstré6m M, Dickman CR (2007) Alien predators are more dangerous than native predators to prey populations. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, Biological Sciences 274(1615): 1237-1243. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2006.0444 Santos DA, Affonso IP, Message HJ, Okada EK, Gomes LC, Bornatowski H, Vitule JRS (2019) Societal perception, impacts and judgment values about invasive freshwater stingrays. Biological Invasions 21(12): 3593-3606. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10530-019-0207 1-0 Predation risk modulates functional responses of non-native crayfish v8 i Schmitz OJ, Krivan V, Ovadia O (2004) Trophic cascades: The primacy of trait-mediated indirect in- teractions. Ecology Letters 7(2): 153-163. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1461-0248.2003.00560.x Seebens H, Blackburn TM, Dyer EE, Genovesi P, Hulme PE, Jeschke JM, Pagad S, Pysek P, Winter M, Arianoutsou M, Bacher S, Blasius B, Brundu G, Capinha C, Celesti-Grapow L, Dawson W, Dullinger S, Fuentes N, Jager H, Kartesz J, Kenis M, Kreft H, Kithn I, Lenzner B, Liebhold A, Mosena A, Moser D, Nishino M, Pearman D, Pergl J, Rabitsch W, Rojas- Sandoval J, Roques A, Rorke S, Rossinelli $, Roy HE, Scalera R, Schindler S, Stajerova K, Tokarska-Guzik B, Van Kleunen M, Walker K, Weigelt P, Yamanaka T, Essl F (2017) No saturation in the accumulation of alien species worldwide. Nature Communications 8(1): e14435. https://doi.org/10.1038/ncomms14435 Sih A, Bolnick DI, Luttbeg B, Orrock JL, Peacor SD, Pintor LM, Preisser E, Rehage JS, Vonesh JR (2010) Predator-prey naiveté, antipredator behavior, and the ecology of predator inva- sions. Oikos 119(4): 610-621. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1600-0706.2009.18039.x Solomon ME (1949) The natural control of animal populations. Journal of Animal Ecology 18(1): 1-35. https://doi.org/10.2307/1578 South J, McCard M, Khosa D, Mofu L, Madzivanzira TC, Dick JTA, Weyl OLF (2019) The effect of prey identity and substrate type on the functional response of a globally invasive crayfish. NeoBiota 52: 9-24. https://doi.org/10.3897/neobiota.52.39245 South J, Madzivanzira TC, Tshali N, Measey J, Weyl OLF (2020) Ina pinch: Mechanisms behind potential biotic resistance toward two invasive crayfish by native African freshwater crabs. Frontiers in Ecology and Evolution 8: 1-72. https://doi.org/10.3389/fevo.2020.00072 Stebbing PD, Watson GJ, Bentley MG (2010) The response to disturbance chemicals and predator odours of juvenile and adult signal crayfish Pacifastacus leniusculus (Dana). Marine and Freshwater Behaviour and Physiology 43(3): 183-195. https://doi.org/10.1080/1023 6244.2010.487301 Stein JN (1970) A Study of the Largemouth Bass Population in Lake Washington. University of Washington, Washington. Taylor NG, Dunn AM (2018) Predatory impacts of alien decapod Crustacea are predicted by functional responses and explained by differences in metabolic rate. Biological Invasions 20(10): 2821-2837. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10530-018-1735-y Turner AM, Mittelbach GG (1990) Predator avoidance and community structure: Interactions among piscivores, planktivores, and plankton. Ecology 71(6): 2241-2254. https://doi. org/10.2307/1938636 Twardochleb LA, Olden JD, Larson ER (2013) A global meta-analysis of the ecological impacts of nonnative crayfish. Freshwater Science 32(4): 1367-1382. https://doi.org/10.1899/12-203. 1 Usio N, Nakata K, Kawai T, Kitano S (2007) Distribution and control status of the invasive signal crayfish (Pacifastacus leniusculus) in Japan. Japanese Journal of Limnology 68(3): 471-482. https://doi.org/10.3739/rikusui.68.47 1 Usio N, Kamiyama R, Saji A, Takamura N (2009) Size-dependent impacts of invasive alien crayfish on a littoral marsh community. Biological Conservation 142(7): 1480-1490. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biocon.2009.02.016 VaefSen S, Hollert H (2015) Impacts of the North American signal crayfish (Pacifastacus lenius- culus) on European ecosystems. Environmental Sciences Europe 27(1): 1-33. https://doi. org/10.1186/s12302-015-0065-2 212 Larissa Faria et al. / NeoBiota 87: 191-212 (2023) Vollmer KL, Gall BG (2014) Complex predator-prey interactions between the rusty crayfish (Orconectes rusticus) and invertebrate and vertebrate prey within their native range. Journal of Freshwater Ecology 29(2): 267-277. https://doi.org/10.1080/02705060.2014.888688 Wagner MJ, Moore PA (2022) Are you scared yet? Variations to cue indices elicit differential prey behavioral responses even when gape-limited predators are relatively small. Canadian Journal of Zoology 100(9): 583-595. https://doi.org/10.1139/cjz-2022-0050 Werner EE, Peacor SD (2003) A review of trait-mediated indirect interactions in eco- logical communities. Ecology 84(5): 1083-1100. https://doi.org/10.1890/0012- 9658(2003)084[1083:AROTII]2.0.CO;2 Westhoff JT, Rosenberger AE (2016) A global review of freshwater crayfish temperature toler- ance, preference, and optimal growth. Reviews in Fish Biology and Fisheries 26(3): 329- 349. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11160-016-9430-5 Wood TC, Moore PA (2020a) Big and bad: How relative predator size and dietary information influence rusty crayfish (Faxonius rusticus) behavior and resource-use decisions. Canadian Journal of Zoology 98(1): 62—72. https://doi.org/10.1139/cjz-2019-0089 Wood TC, Moore PA (2020b) Fine-tuned responses to chemical landscapes: Crayfish use pred- ator odors to assess threats based on relative sizes. Ecosphere 11(9): e03188. https://doi. org/10.1002/ecs2.3188 Wood TC, Kelley RE, Moore PA (2018) Feeding in fear: Indirect effects of predatory fish on macrophyte communities mediated by altered crayfish foraging behaviour. Freshwater Bi- ology 63(12): 1523-1533. https://doi.org/10.1111/fwb.13181 Zanette LY, Clinchy M (2019) Ecology of fear. Current Biology 29(9): R309-R3 13. https://doi. org/10.1016/j.cub.2019.02.042 Zavorka L, Buoro M, Cucherousset J (2018) The negative ecological impacts of a globally introduced species decrease with time since introduction. Global Change Biology 24(9): 4428-4437. https://doi.org/10.1111/gcb.14323 Supplementary material | Data from functional response trials Authors: Larissa Faria, Jean R. S. Vitule, Julian D. Olden Data type: xls Explanation note: Data from functional response trials for each crayfish species under predator treatment and control. Copyright notice: This dataset is made available under the Open Database License (http://opendatacommons.org/licenses/odbl/1.0/). The Open Database License (ODDbL) is a license agreement intended to allow users to freely share, modify, and use this Dataset while maintaining this same freedom for others, provided that the original source and author(s) are credited. Link: https://doi.org/10.3897/neobiota.87.108457.suppl1